Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T02:38:42.170Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Multi-Session Attribution Modification Program for Children with Aggressive Behaviour: Changes in Attributions, Emotional Reaction Estimates, and Self-Reported Aggression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 April 2014

Stephanos P. Vassilopoulos*
Affiliation:
University of Patras, Greece
Andreas Brouzos
Affiliation:
University of Ioannina, Greece
Eleni Andreou
Affiliation:
University of Thessaly, Greece
*
Reprint requests to Stephanos P. Vassilopoulos, Department of Primary Education, University of Patras, Patras 26 110, Greece. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background: Research suggests that aggressive children are prone to over-attribute hostile intentions to peers. Aims: The current study investigated whether this attributional style can be altered using a Cognitive Bias Modification of Interpretations (CBM-I) procedure. Method: A sample of 10–12-year-olds selected for displaying aggressive behaviours was trained over three sessions to endorse benign rather than hostile attributions in response to ambiguous social scenarios. Results: Compared to a test-retest control group (n = 18), children receiving CBM-I (n = 16) were less likely to endorse hostile attributions and more likely to endorse benign attributions in response to a new set of ambiguous social situations. Furthermore, aggressive behaviour scores reduced more in the trained group than in the untrained controls. Children who received attribution training also reported less perceived anger and showed a trend to report more self-control than those in the control group. Conclusions: Implications of these findings are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrett, P., Rapee, R., Dadds, M. and Ryan, S. (1996). Family enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 187203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coie, J. (1990). Teacher Checklist of Social Behavior. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Crick, N. and Dodge, K. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (2006). Translational science in action: hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Developmental Psychopathology, 18, 791814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epps, J. and Kendall, P. C. (1995). Hostile attributional bias in adults. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 159178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gresham, F. and Elliot, S. (1990). The Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Hawkins, K. A. and Cougle, J. R. (2013). Effects of interpretation training on hostile attribution bias and reactivity to interpersonal insult. Behavior Therapy, 44, 479488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudley, C., Britsch, B., Wakefield, W. D., Smith, T., Demorat, M. and Cho, S.-J. (1998). An attribution retraining program to reduce aggression in elementary school students. Psychology in the Schools, 35, 271282.3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ialongo, N. S., Vaden-Kiernan, N. and Kellam, S. (1998). Early peer rejection and aggression: longitudinal relations with adolescent behavior. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 10, 199213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Greca, A. M. and Stone, W. L. (1993). The Social Anxiety Scale for Children-Revised: factor structure and concurrent validity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 1727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathews, A. and Mackintosh, B. (2000). Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 602615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., Huijding, J., Mayer, B. and Hameetman, M. (2008). A space odyssey: experimental manipulation of threat perception and anxiety-related interpretation bias in children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 39, 469480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., Huijding, J., Mayer, B., Remmerswaal, D. and Vreden, S. (2009). Ground control to Major Tom: experimental manipulation of anxiety-related interpretation bias by means of the “space odyssey” paradigm and the effects on avoidance tendencies in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 333340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orpinas, P. and Frankowski, R. (2001). The aggression scale: a self-report measure of aggressive behavior for young adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 5067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vassilopoulos, S. P. and Banerjee, R. (2008). Interpretations and judgments regarding positive and negative social scenarios in childhood social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 870876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vassilopoulos, S. P., Banerjee, R. and Prantzalou, C. (2009). Experimental modification of interpretation bias in socially anxious children: changes in interpretation, anticipated interpersonal anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 10851089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waas, G. A. (1988). Social attributional biases of peer-rejected and aggressive children. Child Development, 59, 969992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilmshurst, L. (2009). Abnormal Child Psychology: a developmental perspective. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.