Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:56:12.313Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do Females with Bulimia Nervosa and Eating Disorder Not Otherwise Specified Have Selective Memory Biases?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 April 2014

Emma Griffith*
Affiliation:
University of Exeter, UK
Willem Kuyken
Affiliation:
University of Exeter, UK
Ed Watkins
Affiliation:
University of Exeter, UK
Alysun Jones
Affiliation:
Avon and Wiltshire Partnership NHS Trust, Bristol, UK
*
Reprint requests to Emma Griffith, Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background: The cognitive model suggests memory biases for weight/shape and food related information could be important in the maintenance of eating disorders. Aims: The current study aims to evaluate this and extend previous research by (a) including females with eating disorder not otherwise specified (EDNOS) as a discreet group; (b) considering whether levels of hunger and the pleasantness of the stimulus words are important in word recall. Method: The study includes three groups of females, 16 with bulimia nervosa, 18 with EDNOS and 17 non-dieting general population controls. All participants completed a self-referential encoding and memory recall task. Results: A main effect of word type (p < .01) with no group by word type interaction or between group difference was found. A priori contrasts indicated that both eating disorder groups recalled significantly more weight/shape and food words compared to all other word categories (p < .01) compared to the control group; with no significant difference found between the eating disorder groups. In relation to the recall of food words, no significant differences were found between groups for levels of hunger. Both eating disorder groups rated the negative weight/shape (p < .01), negative food (p < .01) and neutral body words (p < .01) as more unpleasant than the control group. Conclusions: The implications for cognitive theory and future research are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn., text rev. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Steer, A. A. and Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory: second edition manual. San Antonio, CA: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Crawford, J. R., Besson, J. A. O., Parker, D. M., Sutherland, K. M. and Keen, P. L. (1989). Estimation of premorbid intellectual status in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 313314.Google Scholar
Fairburn, C. G. (1981). A cognitive behavioural approach to the management of bulimia. Psychological Medicine, 11, 707711.Google Scholar
Fairburn, C. G. and Beglin, S. J. (1994). Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self-report questionnaire? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16, 363370.Google Scholar
Fairburn, C. G. and Cooper, Z. (1993). The interview schedule of the Eating Disorders Examination (12th edn). In Fairburn, C. G.. and Wilson, G. T.. (Eds.), Binge Eating: nature, assessment and treatment. (pp. 317360). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fairburn, C. G. and Harrison, P. J. (2003). Eating disorders. The Lancet, 361, 407416.Google Scholar
Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics using SPSS (2nd edn). London: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Grand, S. (1968). Color-word interference. II. An investigation of the role of vocal conflict and hunger in associative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 3140.Google Scholar
Harvey, A., Watkins, E., Mansell, W. and Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive Behavioural Processes Across Psychological Disorders: a transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Pieters, G. and Eelen, P. (1998). Implicit and explicit memory for shape, body weight and food-related words in patients with anorexia and non-dieting controls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 193202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, J. and Cooper, M. J. (2001). Selective memory bias in women with bulimia nervosa and women with depression. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 93102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, M. and Shafran, R. (2004). Information processing biases in eating disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 215238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legenbauer, T., Maul, B., Rühl, I., Kleinstäuber, M. and Hiller, W. (2010). Memory bias for schema-related stimuli in individuals with bulimia nervosa. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 66, 302316.Google Scholar
Lungh, L. G. and Ost, L. G. (1996). Recognition bias for critical faces in social phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 787794.Google Scholar
Nelson, H. E. (1991). National Adult Reading Test (NART) Second Edition Test Manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.Google Scholar
Nikendei, C., Weisbrod, M., Schild, S., Bender, S., Walther, S., Herzog, W., et al. (2008). Anorexia nervosa: selective processing of food-related word and pictorial stimuli in recognition and free recall tests. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41, 439447.Google Scholar
Phillips, L., Tiggemann, M. and Wade, T. (1997). Comparisons of cognitive style in bulimia nervosa and depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 939948.Google Scholar
Rees, L. (2002). Can Collaboration Between Education and Health Professionals Improve the Identification of Young People with Eating Disorders in Schools? A pilot study. Unpublished doctorate thesis, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Exeter, UK.Google Scholar
Ridout, N., Astell, A. J., Reid, I. C., Glen, T. and O’Carroll, R. E. (2003). Memory bias for emotional facial expressions in major depression. Cognitive and Emotion, 17, 101122.Google Scholar
Sebastian, S. B., Williamson, D. A. and Blouin, D. C. (1996). Memory bias for fatness stimuli in the eating disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 275286.Google Scholar
Shafran, R. and Robinson, P. (2004). Thought-shape fusion in eating disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 399407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
University of Western Australia (1981). MRC psycholinguistic database. Retrieved 21 February 2005, from http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/mrcdatabase/uwa_mrc.htmGoogle Scholar
Vitousek, K. M. (1996). The current status of cognitive behavioural models of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In Salkovskis, P. M. (Ed.) Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy. London: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Vitousek, K. B. and Hollon, S. D. (1990). The investigation of schematic content and processing in eating disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 191214.Google Scholar
Wildes, J. E., Simons, A. D. and Marcus, M. D. (2005). Bulimic symptoms, cognitions, and body dissatisfaction in women with major depressive disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 38, 917.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders (2nd ed.) Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Williamson, D. A., White, M. A., York-Crowe, E. and Stewart, T. M. (2004). Cognitive behavioural theories of eating disorders. Behaviour Modification, 28, 711738.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2006). BMI Classification. Retrieved 10 February 2014, from http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.htmlGoogle Scholar
Wuensch, K. L. (2006). One way independent samples analysis of variance. Retrieved June 15, 2006, from www.core.ecu.edu/psyc/wuenschk/docs30/anova1Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.