No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Why language survives as the dominant communication tool: A neurocognitive perspective
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 30 September 2021
Abstract
By focusing on the contributions of subcortical structures, our commentary suggests that the functions of the hippocampus underlying “displacement,” a feature enabling humans to communicate things and situations that are remote in space and time, make language more effective at social bonding. Based on the functions of the basal ganglia and hippocampus, evolutionary trajectory of the subcomponents of music and language in different species will also be discussed.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Brown, S. (2000). The “musilanguage” model of musical evolution. In Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., & Brown, S. (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 271–300). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Buckley, M. J., & Gaffan, D. (2000). The hippocampus, perirhinal cortex, and memory in the monkey. In Bolhuis, J. J. (Ed.), Brain, perception, and memory: Advances in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 279–298). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature, 395(6699), 272–274. https://doi.org/10.1038/26216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Covington, N. V., & Duff, M. C. (2016). Expanding the language network: Direct contributions from the hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(12), 869–870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crystal, J. D., & Smith, A. E. (2014). Binding of episodic memories in the rat. Current Biology, 24(24), 2957–2961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Waal, F., & Ferrari, P. F. (2010). Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition. Trends Cognitive Science, 14, 201–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickerson, B. C., & Eichenbaum, H. (2010). The episodic memory system: Neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 86–104. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ergorul, C., & Eichenbaum, H. (2004). The hippocampus and memory for “what,” “where,” and “when.” Learning & Memory, 11(4), 397–405. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.73304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140091. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, K. L., Gilbertson, K. E., Hrvol, A. J., Nelson, J. C., Seyfer, A. L., Brantner, R. M., & Kamil, A. C. (2013). Differences in relative hippocampus volume and number of hippocampus neurons among five corvid species. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 2013(81), 56–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grahn, J. A. (2009). The role of the basal ganglia in beat perception: Neuroimaging and neuropsychological investigations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 35–45. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04553.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science (New York, N.Y.), 298(5598), 1569–1579. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.298.5598.1569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockett, C. F. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203(3), 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0960-88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, E. D. (2007). Neural systems for vocal learning in birds and humans: A synopsis. Journal of Ornithology, 148(Suppl. 1), S35–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-007-0243-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotz, S. A., Ravignani, A., & Fitch, W. T. (2018). The evolution of rhythm processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(10), 896–910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Ordas, G., Haun, D., Colmenares, F., & Call, J. (2010). Keeping track of time: Evidence for episodic-like memory in great apes. Animal Cognition, 13(2), 331–340. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0282-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen, R. K., Moses, S. N., Riggs, L., & Ryan, J. D. (2012). The hippocampus supports multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and comparison. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravignani, A., Dalla Bella, S., Falk, S., Kello, C. T., Noriega, F., & Kotz, S. A. (2019). Rhythm in speech and animal vocalizations: A cross-species perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1453, 79–98. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, E. R., & Zhang, Q. (2020). A domain-general perspective on the role of the basal ganglia in language and music: Benefits of music therapy for the treatment of aphasia. Brain and Language, 206, 104811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (2001). Episodic memory and common sense: How far apart? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 356(1413), 1505–1515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, E. Q., & Alamri, S. (2016). Revisiting the role of the hippocampus in vocal learning. Ducog 2016, Dubrovnik, Croatia.Google Scholar
Target article
Music as a coevolved system for social bonding
Related commentaries (24)
A boldly comparative approach will strengthen co-evolutionary accounts of musicality's origins
A neurodevelopmental disorders perspective into music, social attention, and social bonding
Beyond “consistent with” adaptation: Is there a robust test for music adaptation?
Clarifying the link between music and social bonding by measuring prosociality in context
Ecological and psychological factors in the cultural evolution of music
Evolutionary linguistics can help refine (and test) hypotheses about how music might have evolved
Human evolution of gestural messaging and its critical role in the human development of music
If it quacks like a duck: The by-product account of music still stands
Is neural entrainment to rhythms the basis of social bonding through music?
Is the MSB hypothesis (music as a coevolved system for social bonding) testable in the Popperian sense?
Isochrony, vocal learning, and the acquisition of rhythm and melody
Music and dance are two parallel routes for creating social cohesion
Music as a social bond in patients with amnesia
Music as a trait in evolutionary theory: A musicological perspective
Not by signalling alone: Music's mosaicism undermines the search for a proper function
Oxytocin as an allostatic agent in the social bonding effects of music
Pre-hunt charade as the cradle of human musicality
Progress without exclusion in the search for an evolutionary basis of music
Rapid dissonant grunting, or, but why does music sound the way it does?
Sex and drugs and rock and roll
Social bonding and music: Evidence from lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex
The evolution of music as artistic cultural innovation expressing intuitive thought symbolically
Where they sing solo: Accounting for cross-cultural variation in collective music-making in theories of music evolution
Why don't cockatoos have war songs?
Author response
Toward a productive evolutionary understanding of music
Toward inclusive theories of the evolution of musicality