Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:03:54.032Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

When nurture becomes nature: Ethnocentrism in studies of human development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 June 2010

David F. Lancy
Affiliation:
Program in Anthropology, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322. [email protected]://www.usu.edu/anthro/davidlancyspages/index.html

Abstract

This commentary will extend the territory claimed in the target article by identifying several other areas in the social sciences where findings from the WEIRD population have been over-generalized. An argument is made that the root problem is the ethnocentrism of scholars, textbook authors, and social commentators, which leads them to take their own cultural values as the norm.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baumrind, D. (1971) Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology Monographs 4(1, pt. 2):1103.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. A., Sharp, D. W., Ciborowski, T., Frankel, F., Kellemu, J. & Lancy, D. F. (1971) The cultural context of learning and thinking. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4):148–53.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R., Inhelder, B., Lavalée, M. & Retschitzki, J. (1978) Naissance de l'Intelligence Chez Infant Baoulè de Côte d'Ivoire. Hans Huber.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1998) Learning a culture the way informants do: Observing, imitating, and participating. Unpublished manuscript, University of California-Los Angeles. Available at: http://www.bec.ucla.edu/papers/learning_culture.htm.Google Scholar
Fouts, H. N. (2005) Families in Central Africa: A comparison of Bofi farmer and forager families. In: Families in global perspective, ed. Roopnarine, J. L., pp. 347–63. Pearson.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1966) On culture and conservation. In: Studies in cognitive growth, ed. Bruner, J. S., Oliver, R. R. & Greenfield, P. M., pp. 225–56. Wiley.Google Scholar
Kelly, M. (1971) Some aspects of conservation of quantity and length in Papua New Guinea. PNG Journal of Education 7(1):5560.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (1983) Cross-cultural studies in cognition and mathematics. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2007) Accounting for the presence/absence of mother-child play. American Anthropologist 109(2):273–84.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. & Grove, M. A. (2010) Learning guided by others. In: The anthropology of learning in childhood, ed. Lancy, D. F., Gaskins, S. & Bock, J., pp. 145–79. Alta-Mira.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. & Madsen, M. C. (1981) Cultural patterns and the social behavior of children: Two studies from Papua New Guinea. Ethos 9:201–16.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. & Strathern, A. J. (1981) Making-twos: Pairing as an alternative to the taxonomic mode of representation. American Anthropologist 81:773–95.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A. (2004) Challenging expert knowledge: Findings from an African study of infant care and development. In: Childhood and adolescence: Cross-cultural perspectives and applications, ed. Gielen, U. P. & Roopnarine, J. L., pp. 149–65. Praeger.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976) Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Madsen, M. C. (1971) Developmental and cross-cultural differences in the cooperative and competitive behavior of young children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 2(4):365–71.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984) Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In: Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and society, ed. Shweder, R. & LeVine, R. L., pp. 276320. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Price-Williams, D., Gordon, W. & Ramirez, M. (1969) Skill and conservation: A study of pottery-making children. Developmental Psychology 1(6):769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarlett, W. G., Naudeau, S., Salonius-Pasternak, D. & Ponte, I. (2005) Children's play. Sage.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1995) The development of pride and shame in toddlers. In: Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride, ed. Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., pp. 237–52. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Strauss, S. & Ziv, M. (2004) Teaching: Ontogenesis, culture, and education. Cognitive Development 19:451–56.Google Scholar
Sung, J. & Hsu, H. C. (2009) Korean mother's attention regulation and referential speech: Associations with language and play in 1-year-olds. International Journal of Behavioral Development 33(5):430–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1983) Interpersonal abilities of infants as generators for transmission of language and culture. In: The behavior of human infants, ed. Oliverio, A., pp. 145–76. Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidal, F. (1994) Piaget before Piaget. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Whittemore, R. D. (1989) Child caregiving and socialization to the Mandinka way: Toward an ethnography of childhood. Doctoral dissertation, University of California–Los Angeles.Google Scholar