Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T09:34:50.579Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What then should we do?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Seth Roberts
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Calif. 94720

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. A. (1973) A theory for the recognition of items from short memorized lists. Psychological Review 80:417–38. [RDL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Bower, G. H. (1973) Human associative memory. Winston. [CPS]Google Scholar
Baer, D. M. (1982) Some recommendations for a modest reduction in the rate of current recommendations for an immodest increase in the rate of exclusive usages of rate as a dependent measure. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee. [SMD]Google Scholar
Bealer, G. (1978) An inconsistency in functionalism. Synthèse 38:332–72. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blough, D. S. (1963) Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: A new method and some data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 6:237–46. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D. S. (1972) Recognition by the pigeon of stimuli varying in two dimensions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 18:345–67. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D. S. (1978) Reaction times of pigeons on a wavelength discrimination task. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 30:163–67. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D. S. (1982) Pigeon perception of letters of the alphabet. Science 218:397–98. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohr, N. (1958) Atomic physics and human knowledge. [MNR, taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breland, K. & Breland, M. (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist 16:681–84. [RM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgman, P. W. (1945) The prospect for intelligence. Yale Review 34:444–61. [LW]Google Scholar
Bunge, M. (1980) The mind-body problem: A psychological approach. Pergamon Press. [MNR]Google Scholar
Bush, R. R. (1960) A survey of mathematical learning theory. In: Developments in mathematical psychology, ed. Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R. & Licklider, J. C. R.. Free Press. [KMS]Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. (1983) L'homme neuronal. Fayard. [MNR]Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. & Danchin, A. (1976) Selective stabilization of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks. Nature 264:705–12. [MNR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1959) A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language 35:2658. [RM, MNR, CPS, rBFS, ES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press. [RM]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1968) Language and mind. Harcourt, Brace and Jovanovich. [MNR]Google Scholar
Craig, W. (1953) On axiomalizability within a system. Journal of Symbolic Logic 18:3032. [ES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, W. (1956) Replacement of auxiliary expressions. Philosophical Review 65:3855. [ES]Google Scholar
Deitz, S. M. (1978) Current status of applied behavior analysis: Science versus technology. American Psychologist 33:805–14. [SMD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deitz, S. M. (1982) Defining applied behavior analysis: An historical analogy. Behavior Analyst 5:5364. [SMD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deitz, S. M. & Arrington, R. L. (1983) Factors confusing language use in the analysis of behavior. 11:117–32. Behaviorism. [SMD]Google Scholar
Eeeles, J. C. (1979) The human mystery. Springer. [MNR]Google Scholar
Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P. & Skinner, B. F. (1980) Symbolic communication between two pigeons (Columba Livia domestica). Science 207:543–45. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1981) The mind-body problem. Scientific American 244:114–23. [SMD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbon, J. (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review 84:279325. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glymour, C. (1979) Theory and evidence. Princeton University Press. [JMN]Google Scholar
Gudmondsson, K. (1983) The emergence of B. F. Skinner's theory of operant behaviour. Ph.D. thesis, University of Western Ontario. [JMN]Google Scholar
Gustavson, C. R., Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G. & Rusiniak, K. W. (1974) Coyote predation control by aversive conditioning. Science 184:581–83. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, N. R. (1969) Perception and discovery. Freeman, Cooper, and Co. [CPS]Google Scholar
Harzem, P. & Miles, T. R. (1978) Conceptual issues in operant psychology. Wiley. [SMD]Google Scholar
Hebb, D. (1949) The organization of behavior. Wiley. [MNR]Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G. (1980) The logical analysis of psychology. In: Readings in philosophy of psychology, vol. 1, ed. Block, N.. Harvard University Press. [RM]Google Scholar
Hooke, R. (1980) Getting people to use statistics properly. American Statistician 34:3942. [LW]Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943) Principles of behavior. D. Appleton-Century. [taBFS, LW]Google Scholar
Hulse, S. H. & O'Leary, D. K. (1982) Serial pattern learning: Teaching an alphabet to rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 8:260–73. [CPS]Google Scholar
Malcolm, N. (1977) Memory and mind. Cornell University Press. [SMD]Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982) Vision. W. H. Freeman and Company. [RM]Google Scholar
Meck, W. H. (1983) Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 9:171201. [SR]Google ScholarPubMed
Mehler, J. (1974) Connaître par desapprentissage. In: L'unité de l'homme, ed. Morin, E. & Piatelli-Palmarini, . Le Seuil. [MNR]Google Scholar
Monod, J. (1970) Le hasard et la necessité. Le Seuil. [MNR]Google Scholar
Morse, W. H. & Skinner, B. F. (1958) Some factors involved in the stimulus control of operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 1:103–7. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, O. H. & Jones, H. M. (1943) Extinction and behavior variability as functions of effortfulness of task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 33:369–86. [tcBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, R. J. (1969) Behaviorism is false. Journal of Philosophy 66:417–51. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, R. J. (1982) The logic of mind. Reidel. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. (1981) The knowledge level. Al Magazine 2:120. [RM]Google Scholar
Neyman, J. & Pearson, E. S. (1967) Joint statistical papers. Cambridge University Press. [FHCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1878) How to make our ideas clear. Popular Science Monthly, 01. [ES]Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1961) Logic of scientific discovery. Science Editions. [ES]Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1969) Science: Conjectures and refutations. In: Conjectures and refutations. Basic Books. [ES]Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. & Eccles, J.-C. (1977) The self and its brain. Springer. [MNR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prelec, D. (1982) Matching, maximizing, and the hyperbolic reinforcement feedback function. Psychological Review 89:189230. [RDL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning, vol. 2, Current research and theory, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [SR]Google Scholar
Richelle, M. (1976) Formal analysis and functional analysis of verbal behavior: Notes on the debate between Chomsky and Skinner (transl. Foster, W. S. & Rondal, J. A.). Behaviorism 4:209–21. [MNR]Google Scholar
Richelle, M. (1977) B. F. Skinner ou le péril behavioriste. Mardaga. [MNR]Google Scholar
Roberts, S. (1981) Isolation of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 7:242–68. [SR]Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, S. (1982) Cross-modal use of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 8:222. [SR]Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, S. & Holder, M. D. (in press) What starts an internal clock? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. [SR]Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1892) Animal intelligence. D. Appleton. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rorty, R. (1979) Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton University Press. [CPS]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1958) “What is learned?” – an empirical enigma. Psychological Review 65:2233. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1961) Ontological induction and the logical typology of scientific variables. Philosophy of Science 28:337–77. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1966) Scaling theory and the nature of measurement. Synthèse, 16:170233. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1967) Conditioned generalization, cognitive set, and the structure of human learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 6:491500. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1970) The art of metascience, or, what should a psychological theory be? In: Toward unification in psychology, ed. Royce, J. R.. Toronto University Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1972) Scientific inference: The myth and the reality. In: Science, psychology and communication, ed. Brown, S. R. & Brenner, D. J.. Teachers College Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1973) Dispositions revisited. Philosophy of Science 40:5974. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1982) Let's dump hypothetico-deductivism for the right reasons. Philosophy of Science 49:637–47. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1984) Dispositions do explain. In: Annals of theoretical psychology, vol. 1, ed. Royce, J. R. & Mos, L. P.. Plenum Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (forthcoming) Do mental systems have a scientific future? [WWR]Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949) The concept of mind. Harper & Row. [SMD]Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974) On going back tonature: A review of Seligman and Hager's Biological boundaries of learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:183–98. [BM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. & Hager, J. L., eds. (1972) Biological boundaries of learning. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [BM]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1975) Perspectives on the behavioral unit: Choice behavior in animals. In: Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, ed. Estes, W. K., vol. 2. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [CPS]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1976) Organization in memory and behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 26:113–30. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimp, C. P. (in press, a) Relations between memory and operant behavior, according to AL. Canadian Journal of Psychology. [CPS]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (in press, b) Timing, learning and forgetting. In: Timing, ed. Gibbon, J. & Allen, L.. New York Academy of Sciences. [CPS]Google Scholar
Sidman, M. (1978) Remarks. Behaviorism 6:265–68. [SMD]Google Scholar
Sidman, M. (1979) Remarks. Behaviorism 7:123–26. [SMD]Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1979) The role of conditioning in drug tolerance and addietion. In: Psychopathology in animals: Research and treatment implications, ed. Keehn, J. D.. Academic Press. [SR]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1931) The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology 5:427–58. [JMN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1932) Drive and reflex strength, I. Journal of General Psychology 6:2237. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1935) The generic nature of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology 12:4065. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms,. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan. [WWR, ES]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts. (MNR, rBFS, ES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1966) Operant behavior. In: Operant behavior: Areas of research and applications, ed. Honig, W. K.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [CPS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1969) Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Prentice-Hall. [SMD, KMS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1972) Cumulative record: A selection of papers. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [JMN]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1973) Beyond freedom and dignity. 1st ed. 1971 Penguin. [BM]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1974) About behaviorism. Alfred Knopf. [WWR, rBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1979) The shaping of a behaviorist: Part two of an autobiography. Alfred Knopf. [JMN]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. & Vaughan, M. E. (1983) Enjoy old age. Norton. [MNR]Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1957) On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review 64:153–81. [JTT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Straub, R. O., Seidenberg, M. S., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S. (1979) Serial learning in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32:137–48. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suppes, P. (1975) From behaviorism to neobehaviorism. Theory and Decision 6:269–86. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, J. T. (1972) Some results concerning the identifiability of parallel and serial processes. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 25:168199. [JTT]Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T. & Ashby, F. G. (1983) The stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge University Press. [JTT]Google Scholar
Tukey, J. W. (1980) Teaching of statistics: We need both exploratory and confirmatory. American Statistician 34:2325. [LW]Google Scholar
Tuomela, R., ed. (1978) Dispositions. Reidel. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallis, W. A. & Roberts, H. V. (1956) Statistics: A new approach. Free-Press. [LW]Google Scholar
Westby, G. (1966) Psychology today: Problems and directions. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society 19(65). [taBFS]Google Scholar
Whewell, W. (1967) History of the inductive sciences. 1st ed. 1837. Cass. [MLS]Google Scholar
Wilson, E. B. (1952) An introduction to scientific research. McGraw-Hill. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953) Philosophical investigations. Macmillan. [SMD, CPS]Google Scholar