Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:26:12.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Top-down versus bottom-up perspectives on clinically significant memory reconsolidation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Terry Marks-Tarlow
Affiliation:
Teaching Faculty, Reiss Davis Child Study Center, Los Angeles, CA [email protected]
Jaak Panksepp
Affiliation:
Department of Integrative Physiology and Neuroscience, College of Veterinary Medicine, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164. [email protected]

Abstract

Lane et al. are right: Troublesome memories can be therapeutically recontextualized. Reconsolidation of negative/traumatic memories within the context of positive/prosocial affects can facilitate diverse psychotherapies. Although neural mechanisms remain poorly understood, we discuss how nonlinear dynamics of various positive affects, heavily controlled by primal subcortical networks, may be critical for optimal benefits.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Burgdorf, J., Panksepp, J. & Moskal, J. R. (2011) Frequency-modulated 50kHz ultrasonic vocalizations: A tool for uncovering the molecular substrates of positive affect. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35:1831–36.Google Scholar
Ecker, B., Ticic, R. & Hulley, L. (2012) Unlocking the emotional brain: Eliminating symptoms at their roots using memory reconsolidation. Routledge.Google Scholar
Karasu, T. B. (1986) The specificity versus nonspecificity dilemma: Toward identifying therapeutic change agents. The American Journal of Psychiatry 143:687–95.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. J., Misanin, J. R. & Miller, R. R. (1968) Recovery of memory following amnesia. Nature 220:704705.Google Scholar
Marks-Tarlow, T. (2008) Psyche's veil: Psychotherapy, fractals and complexity. Routledge.Google Scholar
Marks-Tarlow, T. (2012) Clinical intuition in psychotherapy: The neurobiology of embodied change. Norton.Google Scholar
Marks-Tarlow, T. (2014) Awakening clinical intuition: An experiential workbook for psychotherapists. Norton.Google Scholar
Marks-Tarlow, T. (in press) The deep structure of play within psychotherapy. In: Handbook of play, ed. Johnson, J. & Eberle, S.. Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Merker, B. (2007) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30:63134.Google Scholar
Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R. & Lewis, D. J. (1968) Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 160:554–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, L., Hupbach, A., Gomez, R. & Newman-Smith, K. (2012) Memory formation, consolidation and transformation. Neuroscience Behavioral Review 36(7):1640–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nader, K., Hardt, O. & Lanius, R. (2013) Memory as a new therapeutic target. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 2013:475–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998) Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2008) PLAY, ADHD and the construction of the social brain: Should the first class each day be recess? American Journal of Play 1:5579.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011) Empathy and the laws of affect. Science 334:1358–59.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., ed. (2004) A textbook of biological psychiatry. Wiley.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Biven, L. (2012) Archaeology of mind: The neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. Norton.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Wright, J., Döbrössy, M. D., Schlaepfer, T. E. & Coenen, V. A. (2014) Affective neuroscience strategies for understanding and treating depressions: From preclinical models to novel therapeutics. Clinical Psychological Science 2:472–94.Google Scholar
Schlaepfer, T. E., Bewernick, B. H., Kayser, S., Mädler, B. & Coenen, V. A. (2013) Rapid effects of deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant major depression. Biological Psychiatry 73:1204–12.Google Scholar
Schwabe, L., Nader, K. & Pruessner, J. C. (2014) Reconsolidation of human memory: Brain mechanisms and clinical relevance. Biological Psychiatry 76(4):274–80.Google Scholar
Shevrin, H., Panksepp, J., Brakel, L. L. A. W. & Snodgrass, M. (2012) Subliminal affect valence words change conscious mood potency but not valence: Is this evidence for unconscious valence affect? Brain Science 2:504522.Google Scholar
Solms, M. & Panksepp, J. (2012) The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: Neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sciences 2(2):147–75.Google Scholar
Winkielman, P. & Berridge, K. (2004) Unconscious emotion. Current directions in Psychological Science 13(3):120–23.Google Scholar