Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T04:28:55.972Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Three cognitive mechanisms for knowledge tracking

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2021

Dora Kampis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 2A, 1353Copenhagen, [email protected]
Gergely Csibra
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, Central European University, Október 6. u. 7, 1051Budapest, Hungary. [email protected]

Abstract

We welcome Phillips et al.'s proposal to separate the understanding of “knowledge” from that of “beliefs.” We argue that this distinction is best specified at the level of the cognitive mechanisms. Three distinct mechanisms are discussed: tagging one's own representations with those who share the same reality; representing others' representations (metarepresenting knowledge); and attributing dispositions to provide useful information.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15(5), 611617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horschler, D. J., Santos, L. R., & MacLean, E. L. (2019). Do non-human primates really represent others’ ignorance? A test of the awareness relations hypothesis. Cognition, 190, 7280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kampis, D., & Kovács, Á. M. (2020). Seeing the world from others’ perspective: 14-month-olds show altercentric modulation effects by others’ beliefs. (psyarxiv.com/an7h3).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, Á. M., Tauzin, T., Téglás, E., Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2014). Pointing as epistemic request: 12-month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy, 19(6), 543557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibly to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 18301834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Developmental Science, 12(4), 521535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. (2016). What cognitive representations support primate theory of mind?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(5), 375382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillow, B. H. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47(1), 116129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61(4), 973982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Dietrich, N. (2006). Two- to four-year-old children's differentiation of knowing and guessing in a non-verbal task. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3(3), 222237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, J. N., Glynn, D. D., Phillips, B. C. & Hauser, M. D. (2007). The perception of rational, goal-directed action in nonhuman primates. Science, 317(5843), 14021405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolley, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (1993). Origin and truth: Young children's understanding of imaginary mental representations. Child Development, 64(1), 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed