No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
There's reconstruction, and there's behavior control
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986
References
Allport, D. A. (1975) The state of cognitive psychology: A critical notice of W. G. Chase (ed.), visual information processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27:141–52. [HJE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakan, D. (1966) Behaviorism and American urbanization. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 2:5–28. [LK]3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, W. M. (1973) The correlation-based law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:137–53. [HR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benacerraf, P. & Putnam, H., eds. (1984) Philosophy of mathematics, 2nd ed.Cambridge University Press. [MJM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergmann, G. (1953) Theoretical psychology. Annual Review of Psychology 4:435–58. [RS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergmann, G. (1956) The contribution of John B. Watson. Psychological Review 63:265–76. [JAD, RE]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergmann, G. & Spence, K. W. (1941) Operationism and theory in psychology. Psychological Review 48:1–14. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bever, T. G., Fodor, J. A. & Garrett, M. (1968) A formal limit of associationism. In: Verbal behavior and general theory, ed. Dixon, T. R. & Morton, D. L.. Prentice-Hall. [MNB]Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. (1971) Elicitation, reinforcement and stimulus control. In: The nature of reinforcement, ed. Glaser, R.. Academic Press. [HR]Google Scholar
Chaplin, J. & Krawiec, T. (1968) Systems and theories of psychology, 2nd ed.Holt. [JAD]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior: Language 35:26–58. [MNB]Google Scholar
Costall, A. P. (1984) Are theories of perception necessary? A review of Gibson's The ecological approach to visual perception. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior 41:109–15. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983) The cognitive challenge in historical perspective. Paper presented at annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Nashville, Tenn. [JAD]Google Scholar
Dunlap, K. (1916) Thought-content and feeling. Psychological Review 23:49–70. [RE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, R. (1984) The case for praxics. The Behavior Analyst 7:101–19. [RE]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, R. (1985a) Animal cognition as the praxist views it. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 9:623–30. [RE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, R. (1985b) Further comments on praxics: Why the devotion to behaviorism? The Behavioral Analyst 8:269–71. [RE]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, R. (in press a) In the yellow wood (concluding chapter). In: B. F. Skinner: Consensus and controversy, ed. Modgil, S. & Modgil, C.. Falmer Press. [RE]Google Scholar
Epstein, R. (in press b) Reflections on thinking in animals. In Language, cognition, and consciousness: Integrative levels, ed. Greenberg, G. & Tobach, E.. Erlbaum Associates. [RE]Google Scholar
Estes, W. K., Koch, MacCorquodale, Meehl, Mueller, Schoenfeld, & Verplanck, , eds. (1954) Modern learning theory. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [RS]Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1979) The structure and measurement of intelligence. Springer. [HJE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, R. (1983) The social applications of Pavlovian theories. The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Studies 18:117–25. [HJE]Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, M. W. (1985) Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. Plenum Press. [HJE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. F. (1974) The psychology of language. McGraw-Hill. [RS]Google Scholar
French, A. P., ed. (1979) Einstein: A centenary volume. Harvard University Press. [MJM]Google Scholar
Fulker, D. W. (1981) The genetic and environmental architecture of psychoticism, extraversion and neuroticism. In: A model for personality, ed. Eysenck, H. J.. Springer. [HJE]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1969) Method and theory in the study of avoidance. Psychological Review 76:49–70. [HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970) On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13:243–66. [HR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, C. L. (1943) Principles of Behavior: An Introduction to Behavior Theory. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [aCEZ, HHK, WWR]Google Scholar
Hunter, W. S. (1925) General anthroponomy and its systematic problems. American Journal of Psychology 36:286–302. [RE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, H. H. (1952) “What is learned?” – A theoretical blind alley. Psychological Review 59:269–77. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, H. H. (1981) Psychology: A science of conflict. Oxford University Press. [HHK]Google Scholar
Kendler, H. H. (1984) Evolutions or revolutions. In: Psychology in the 1990's, ed. Lagerspetz, K. M. J. & Niemi, P.. North Holland. [HHK]Google Scholar
Kendler, H. H. (1985) Behaviorism and psychology: An uneasy alliance. In: A Century of psychology as science, ed. Koch, S. & Leary, D.. McGraw-Hill. [HHK]Google Scholar
Kendler, H. H. (1987) Historical foundations of modern psychology. Dorsey Press. [HHK]Google Scholar
Krasner, L. & Houts, A. C. (1984) A study of the “value” systems of behavioral scientists. American Psychologist 38:840–50. [LK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962) The structure of scientific revolutions. University of Chicago Press. [CPS]Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1937) Prolegomena to praxiology. The Journal of Psychology 4:1–22. [RE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leahey, T. M. (1980) A history of psychology: Main currents in psychological thought. Prentice-Hall. [JAD]Google Scholar
Lloyd, K. E. (1985) Behavioral anthropology: A review of Marvin Harris's Cultural materialsm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 43:279–87. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logue, A. W. (1985) The growth of behaviorism. In: Points of view in the modern history of psychology, ed. Buxton, C. E.. Academic Press. [JAD]Google Scholar
MacCorquodale, K. & Meehl, P. E. (1948) On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. Psychological Review 55:95–107. [rCEZ, WWR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, M. J. (1976) Scientist as subject: The psychological imperative. Ballinger. [LK]Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1979) Emotion. In: The first century of experimental psychology, ed. Hearst, E.. Erlbaum Associates. [HHK]Google Scholar
Medin, D. L. (1975) A theory of context in discrimination learning. In: The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, ed. Bower, G. H.. Academic Press. [CPS]Google Scholar
Medin, D. L. & Smith, E. E. (1984) Concepts and concept formation. Annual Review of Psychology 35:113–38. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. Holt. [HHK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G. L. & Medin, D. L. (1985) The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review 92:289–316. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nevin, J. A. (1969) Interval reinforcement of choice behavior in discrete trials. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:875–86. [HR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olweus, D., Block, J. & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1986) Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior. Academic Press. [HJE]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1984) Neural systems and cognitive processes. In: Psychology in the 1990's, ed. Lagerspetz, K. M. J. & Niemi, P.. North Holland. [HHK]Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1971) Catching up with common sense or two sides of a generalization: Reinforcement and punishment. In: The nature of reinforcement, ed. Glaser, R.. Academic Press. [HR]Google Scholar
Rachlin, H. (1985) Pain and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:43–83. [HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachlin, H., Battalio, R., Kagel, J. & Green, L. (1981) Maximization theory in behavioral psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:371–88. [HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S., eds. (1984) Animal cognition. Erlbaum Associates. [CPS]Google Scholar
Rorty, R. (1979) Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton University Press. [MJM, CPS]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1972) Scientific inference: The myth and the reality. In: Science, psychology, and communication: Essays honoring William Stephenson, ed. Brown, S. R. & Brenner, D. J.. Teachers College Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1984) Dispositions do explain; or, picking up the pieces after Hurricane Walter. Annals of Theoretical Psychology 1:205–23. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanford, F. H. (1951) Across the Secretary's desk: Notes on the future of psychology as a profession. American Psychologist 6:74–76. [JAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1984a) Timing, learning, and forgetting. In: Timing and time perception, ed. Gibbon, J. & Allan, L.. New York Academy of Sciences. [CPS]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1984b) Relations between memory and operant behavior, according to an associative learner (AL). Canadian Journal of Psychology 38:269–84. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1984c) Cognition, behavior, and the experimental analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior 42:407–20. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1945) The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review 52:270–77. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1948a) “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology 38:168–72. [HR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193–216. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1966) What is the experimental analysis of behavior? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior 9:213–18. [aGEZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperling, G. (1960) The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs 74 [Whole No. 498.] [MNB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1979) Operant behavior as adaptation to constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology; General 108:48–67. [HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timberlake, W. & Allison, J. (1974) Response deprivation: An empirical approach to instrumental performance. Psychological Review 81:146–64. [HR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1936) Operational behaviorism and current trends in psychology. In: Proceedings of the twenty-fifth anniversary celebration of the inauguration of graduate studies, the University of Southern California, ed. Hill, H. W.. University of Southern California. [WWR]Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985) How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist 40:385–98. [HHK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vargas, E. A. (1985) Cultural contingencies: A review of Marvin Harris's Cannibals and kings. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 43:419–28. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913) Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review 20:158–77. [JAD, RE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. & McDougall, W. (1928) The battle of behaviorism: An exposition and an exposure. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner. [RE]Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. & Rayner, R. (1920) Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 3:1–14. [HJE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953) Philosophical investigations, trans. Anscombe, G. E. M.. Macmillan. [MJM, CPS]Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1978) Remarks on the foundations of mathematics, revised edition. MIT Press. [MJM]Google Scholar
Wolfle, D. (1947) Annual report of the Executive Secretary. American Psychologist 2:516–20. [JAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuriff, G. E. (1985) Behaviorism: A conceptual reconstruction. Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar