Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref.
Kurian, George
1987.
Preconscious semantic processing: Why and how?.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 04,
p.
766.
Holender, Daniel
1987.
Semantic activation without conscious identification: Can progress be made?.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 04,
p.
768.
Wilding, J. M.
1987.
Semantic effects without awareness: Dichotic listening and dichoptic viewing.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 04,
p.
767.
Groeger, J. A.
1987.
On not knowing the meanings of words we can detect: Crucial qualitative differences.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 04,
p.
765.
Segui, Juan
and
Beauvillain, Cécile
1987.
Why limit the availability of a prime-word in the study of automatic contextual facilitation?.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 04,
p.
766.
Synodinos, Nicolaos E.
1988.
Review and appraisal of subliminal perception within the context of signal detection theory.
Psychology & Marketing,
Vol. 5,
Issue. 4,
p.
317.
Kemp-Wheeler, S. M.
and
Hill, A. B.
1988.
Semantic Priming without Awareness: Some Methodological Considerations and Replications.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A,
Vol. 40,
Issue. 4,
p.
671.
REINGOLD, EYAL M.
and
MERIKLE, PHILIP M.
1990.
On the Inter‐relatedness of Theory and Measurement in the Study of Unconscious Processes.
Mind & Language,
Vol. 5,
Issue. 1,
p.
9.
Reber, Arthur S
1992.
The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective.
Consciousness and Cognition,
Vol. 1,
Issue. 2,
p.
93.
Schwarz, Wolfgang
and
Mecklinger, Axel
1995.
Relationship between flanker identifiability and compatibility effect.
Perception & Psychophysics,
Vol. 57,
Issue. 7,
p.
1045.
Haase, Steven J.
Theios, John
and
Jenison, Rick
1999.
A signal detection theory analysis of an unconscious perception effect.
Perception & Psychophysics,
Vol. 61,
Issue. 5,
p.
986.
Bernat, Edward
Bunce, Scott
and
Shevrin, Howard
2001.
Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing.
International Journal of Psychophysiology,
Vol. 42,
Issue. 1,
p.
11.
Bernat, Edward
Shevrin, Howard
and
Snodgrass, Michael
2001.
Subliminal visual oddball stimuli evoke a P300 component.
Clinical Neurophysiology,
Vol. 112,
Issue. 1,
p.
159.
Wong, P.S
Bernat, E
Snodgrass, M
and
Shevrin, H
2004.
Event-related brain correlates of associative learning without awareness.
International Journal of Psychophysiology,
Vol. 53,
Issue. 3,
p.
217.
Logvinenko, A.D.
Byth, W.
and
Vityaev, E.E.
2004.
In search of an elusive hard threshold: a test of observer’s ability to order sub-threshold stimuli.
Vision Research,
Vol. 44,
Issue. 3,
p.
287.
Erdelyi, Matthew Hugh
2004.
Subliminal perception and its cognates: Theory, indeterminacy, and time.
Consciousness and Cognition,
Vol. 13,
Issue. 1,
p.
73.
Reingold, Eyal M.
2004.
Unconscious perception and the classic dissociation paradigm: A new angle?.
Perception & Psychophysics,
Vol. 66,
Issue. 5,
p.
882.
Snodgrass, Michael
Bernat, Edward
and
Shevrin, Howard
2004.
Unconscious perception: A model-based approach to method and evidence.
Perception & Psychophysics,
Vol. 66,
Issue. 5,
p.
846.
Williams, Leanne M.
Liddell, Belinda J.
Rathjen, Jennifer
Brown, Kerri J.
Gray, Jeffrey
Phillips, Mary
Young, Andy
and
Gordon, Evian
2004.
Mapping the time course of nonconscious and conscious perception of fear: An integration of central and peripheral measures.
Human Brain Mapping,
Vol. 21,
Issue. 2,
p.
64.
Haase, Steven J.
and
Fisk, Gary D.
2004.
Valid distinctions between conscious and unconscious perception?.
Perception & Psychophysics,
Vol. 66,
Issue. 5,
p.
868.