Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:23:04.174Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The other side of the coin: Intersexual selection and the expression of emotions to signal youth or maturity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2009

George A. Lozano
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, Tartu University, 51014 Tartu, Estonia. [email protected]

Abstract

Vigil summarizes sex-related differences in emotivity, and presents a psychological model based on the restrictive assumption that responses to stimuli are dichotomous. The model uses for support the concept of intrasexual selection, but ignores intersexual selection. An alternative hypothesis might be that emotivity signals age: maturity in men and youth in women. Integration requires considering all evolutionary biology, not just agreeable concepts.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersson, M. (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnqvist, G. & Rowe, L. (2005) Sexual conflict. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (1986) Despotism and differential reproduction: A Darwinian view of history. Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (1993) Sex, succession, and stratification in the first six civilizations: How powerful men reproduced, passed power on to their sons, and used power to defend their wealth, women, and children. In: Social stratification and socioeconomic inequality, vol. 1: A comparative biosocial analysis, ed. Ellis, L., 3774. Praeger.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2003) The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating, revised edition.Basic Books.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872) The expression of the emotions in man and animals. John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helle, S., Lummaa, V. & Jokela, J. (2008) Marrying women 15 years younger maximized men's evolutionary fitness in historical Sami. Biology Letters 4:7577.Google Scholar
Leary, T. (1957) Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. Ronald.Google Scholar
Low, B. S. (2001) Why sex matters: A Darwinian look at human behavior. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Majerus, M. E. N. (2003) Sex wars: Genes, bacteria, and biased sex ratios. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Maynard-Smith, J. & Harper, D. (2003) Animal signals. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murstein, B. (1986) Paths to marriage. Sage.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. & Ellsworth, P. C. (2009) Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. American Psychologist 64:129–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. (2005) The evolution of animal communication: Reliability and deception in signalling systems. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology 46:3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971, ed. Campbell, B., pp. 136–79. Aldine.Google Scholar