Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T05:47:23.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The morality of martyrdom and the stigma of suicide

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2014

Joshua Rottman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215. [email protected]@bu.eduwww.bu.edu/childcognition
Deborah Kelemen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215. [email protected]@bu.eduwww.bu.edu/childcognition

Abstract

While primarily identifying similarities between suicide terrorists and other suicidal individuals, Lankford also notes differences in how their actions are morally evaluated. Specifically, “conventional” suicide is stigmatized in a way that suicide terrorism is not. We identify the root of this condemnation, showing that suicide is intuitively considered impure and disgusting, and discuss implications of this purity-based stigma.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atran, S. (2003) Genesis of suicide terrorism. Science 299:1534–39. Available at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/299/5612/1534.full Google Scholar
Bering, J. M. (2011) The belief instinct: The psychology of souls, destiny, and the meaning of life. Norton.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004) Descartes' baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2012) Religion, morality, evolution. Annual Review of Psychology 63(1):179–99. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100334.Google Scholar
Brandt, M. J. & Reyna, C. (2011) The chain of being: A hierarchy of morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6(5):428–46. doi:10.1177/1745691611414587.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1897/1979) Suicide, trans. Spaulding, J. A. & Simpson, G.. The Free Press. (Original work published 1897).Google Scholar
Emmons, N. A. & Kelemen, D. (in press) The development of children's pre-life reasoning: Evidence from two cultures. Child Development.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. (1978) The Gallup poll: Public opinion 1971–1977, vol. 1. Scholarly Resources.Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R. & Maitner, A. T. (2013) Angry at the unjust, scared of the powerful: Emotional responses to terrorist threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 39(8) 1069–82.Google Scholar
Ginges, J., Hansen, I. G. & Norenzayan, A. (2009) Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science 20(2) 224–30. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02270.x.Google Scholar
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S. & Ditto, P. H. (2011) Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101(2):366–85. doi:10.1037/a0021847.Google Scholar
Gray, K., Young, L. & Waytz, A. (2012) Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry 23(2):101–24. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2012) The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H. & Dias, M. G. (1993) Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65(4):p613–28.Google Scholar
Harris, L. T. & Fiske, S. T. (2006) Dehumanizing the lowest of the low. Psychological Science 17(10):p847–53.Google Scholar
Haslam, N. (2006) Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10(3):p252–64.Google Scholar
Joiner, T. (2010) Myths about suicide. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H. & Haidt, J. (2012) Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially Purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality 46(2):184–94. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006.Google Scholar
Lankford, A. (2010) Suicide terrorism as a socially approved form of suicide. Crisis: The Journal of Crisis Intervention and Suicide Prevention 31(6):287–89. doi:10.1027/0227-5910/a000073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lankford, A. (2013c) The myth of martyrdom: What really drives suicide bombers, rampage shooter, and other self-destructive killers. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Parkinson, C., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Koralus, P. E., Mendelovici, A., McGeer, V. & Wheatley, T. (2011) Is morality unified? Evidence that distinct neural systems underlie moral judgments of harm, dishonesty, and disgust. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23(10):3162–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rottman, J., Kelemen, D., & Young, L. (in press) Purity matters more than harm in moral judgments of suicide: Response to Gray. Cognition.Google Scholar
Rottman, J., Kelemen, D. & Young, L. (2014) Tainting the soul: Purity concerns predict moral judgments of suicide. Cognition 130(2):217–26.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S. & Haidt, J. (1999) The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76(4):574–86.Google Scholar
Russell, P. S. & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2011) Moral anger is more flexible than moral disgust. Social Psychological and Personality Science 2(4):360–64. doi:10.1177/1948550610391678.Google Scholar
Russell, P. S., Piazza, J. & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2013) CAD revisited: Effects of the word moral on the moral relevance of disgust (and other emotions). Social Psychological and Personality Science 4(1):6268. doi:10.1177/1948550612442913.Google Scholar
Seidel, A. & Prinz, J. (2013) Sound morality: Irritating and icky noises amplify judgments in divergent moral domains. Cognition 127(1):15. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.004.Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W. & Mullen, E. (2004) Political tolerance and coming to psychological closure following the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks: An integrative approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30(6):743–56. doi:10.1177/0146167204263968.Google Scholar
Young, L. & Saxe, R. (2011) When ignorance is no excuse: Different roles for intent across moral domains. Cognition 120(2):202–14. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.005.Google Scholar