Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:04:37.582Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Human female exogamy is supported by cross-species comparisons: Cause to recognise sex differences in societal policy?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2009

Guy Madison
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Umeå University, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden. [email protected]://www.psy.umu.se/staff/guy_madison_eng.html

Abstract

A sex difference in the tendency to outbreed (female exogamy) is a premise for the target article's proposed framework, which receives some support by being shared with chimpanzees but not with more distantly related primates. Further empirical support is provided, and it is suggested that recognition of sex differences might improve effective fairness, taking sexual assault as a case in point.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, R. D. (1990) How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. University of Michigan Museum of Zoology Special Publication 1:138.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Silk, J. B. (2006) How humans evolved. Norton.Google Scholar
Chivers, M. & Bailey, J. M. (2005) A sex difference in features that elicit genital response. Biological Psychology 70:115–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chivers, M., Rieger, G., Latty, E. & Bailey, J. M. (2004) A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science 15:736–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chivers, M., Seto, M. C. & Blanchard, R. (2007) Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to sexual activities versus gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93:1108–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A. & Ahluvalia, J. (2001) Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development 23:113–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeFruyt, F. (1997) Gender and individual differences in crying. Personality and Individual Differences 22:937–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, D. M., Mok, D. S. & Briere, J. (2004) Adult sexual assault: Prevalence, symptomatology, and sex differences in the general population. Journal of Traumatic Stress 17:203–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ember, C. R. (1978) Myths about hunter-gatherers. Ethnology 17:439–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraemer, D. L. & Hastrup, J. L. (1986) Crying in natural settings: Global estimates, self-monitored frequencies, depression and sex differences in an undergraduate population. Behaviour Research and Therapy 24:371–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S. & Raggatt, P. (2002) Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behavior and Development 25:327–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCown, W., Keiser, R., Mulhearn, S. & Williamson, D. (1997) The role of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music. Personality and Individual Differences 23:543–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKibbin, W. F., Shackelford, T. L., Goetz, A. T. & Starratt, V. T. (2008) Why do men rape? An evolutionary psychological perspective. Review of General Psychology 12:8697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. & Watts, D. P. (2005) Correlates of territorial boundary patrol behaviour in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 70:1079–86.Google Scholar
Nater, U., Abbruzzese, E., Krebs, M. & Ehlert, U. (2006) Sex differences in emotional and psychophysiological responses to musical stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology 62:300308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordenström, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson, A. & Wedell, A. (2002) Sex-typed toy play behavior correlates with the degree of prenatal androgen exposure assessed by CYP21 genotype in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 87:5119–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pusey, A. (1979) Inter-community transfer of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park. In: The great apes, ed. Hamburg, D. & McCown, E., pp. 465–79. Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
Pusey, A., Williams, J. & Goodall, J. (1997) The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science 277:828–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Servin, A., Nordenström, A., Larsson, A. & Bohlin, G. (2003) Prenatal androgens and gender-typed behavior: A study of girls with mild and severe forms of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology 39:440–50.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R. & Palmer, C. T. (2000) A natural history of rape: Biological bases of sexual coercion. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Videan, E. N., Fritz, J., Howell, S. & Murphy, J. (2007) Effects of two types and two genre [sic] of music on social behavior in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science 46:6670.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. L. & Wrangham, R. W. (2003) Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology 32:363–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. & Wilson, M. L. (2004) Collective violence. Comparisons between youths and chimpanzees. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1036:233–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed