Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T16:39:03.558Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2009

Peter Carruthers
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, University of Maryland, College Park, MD [email protected]://www.philosophy.umd.edu/Faculty/pcarruthers/

Abstract

Four different accounts of the relationship between third-person mindreading and first-person metacognition are compared and evaluated. While three of them endorse the existence of introspection for propositional attitudes, the fourth (defended here) claims that our knowledge of our own attitudes results from turning our mindreading capacities upon ourselves. Section 1 of this target article introduces the four accounts. Section 2 develops the “mindreading is prior” model in more detail, showing how it predicts introspection for perceptual and quasi-perceptual (e.g., imagistic) mental events while claiming that metacognitive access to our own attitudes always results from swift unconscious self-interpretation. This section also considers the model's relationship to the expression of attitudes in speech. Section 3 argues that the commonsense belief in the existence of introspection should be given no weight. Section 4 argues briefly that data from childhood development are of no help in resolving this debate. Section 5 considers the evolutionary claims to which the different accounts are committed, and argues that the three introspective views make predictions that are not borne out by the data. Section 6 examines the extensive evidence that people often confabulate when self-attributing attitudes. Section 7 considers “two systems” accounts of human thinking and reasoning, arguing that although there are introspectable events within System 2, there are no introspectable attitudes. Section 8 examines alleged evidence of “unsymbolized thinking”. Section 9 considers the claim that schizophrenia exhibits a dissociation between mindreading and metacognition. Finally, section 10 evaluates the claim that autism presents a dissociation in the opposite direction, of metacognition without mindreading.

Type
Main Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiello, L. & Wheeler, P. (1995) The expensive tissue hypothesis. Current Anthropology 36:199221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. (1995) Learning and memory: An integrated approach. Wiley.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. L. & Perlis, D. (2005a) Logic, self-awareness and self-improvement: The metacognitive loop and the problem of brittleness. Journal of Logic and Computation 15:2140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1988) A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1997) In the theatre of consciousness. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baars, B. J. (2002) The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Science 6:4752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baars, B. J. (2003) How brain reveals mind: Neuroimaging supports the central role of conscious experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10:100–14.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J., Ramsoy, T. & Laureys, S. (2003) Brain, consciousness, and the observing self. Trends in Neurosciences 26:671–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D. & Adlam, A. (2001) Working memory and the control of action: Evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130:641–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayne, T. & Pacherie, E. (2007) Narrators and comparators: The architecture of agentive self-awareness. Synthese 159:475–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begg, I., Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R. & Sanvito, J. (1989) Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory and Language 28:610–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. (1967) Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review 74:183200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bem, D. (1972) Self-perception theory. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 6, ed. Berkowitz, L.. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Benjamin, A. & Bjork, R. (1996) Retrieval fluency as a metacognitive index. In: Implicit memory and metacognition, ed. Reder, L.. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Beran, M., Smith, J., Redford, J. & Washburn, D. (2006) Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) monitor uncertainty during numerosity judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 32:111–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Birch, S. A. J. & Bloom, P. (2004) Understanding children's and adult's limitations in mental state reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Science 8:255–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, S. A. J. & Bloom, P. (2007) The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false beliefs. Psychological Science 18:382–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S., Wolpert, D. & Frith, C. (1998) Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience 1:635–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. (1995) A confusion about the function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:227–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosco, F., Friedman, O. & Leslie, A. (2006) Recognition of pretend and real actions in play by 1- and 2-year-olds: Early success and why they fail. Cognitive Development 21:310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brasil-Neto, J., Pascual-Leone, A., Valls-Solé, J., Cohen, L. & Hallett, M. (1992) Focal transcranial magnetic stimulation and response bias in a forced choice task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55:964–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bratman, M. (1987) Intentions, plans, and practical reason. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bratman, M. (1999) Faces of intention: Selected essays on intention and agency. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briñol, P. & Petty, R. (2003) Overt head movements and persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84:1123–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. & Whiten, A., eds. (1988) Machiavellian intelligence I: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. & Whiten, A., eds. (1997) Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J. & Carpenter, M. (2001) Do apes and children know what they have seen? Animal Cognition 4:207–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12:187–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P. (1996a) Autism as mind-blindness. In: Theories of Theories of Mind, ed. Carruthers, P. & Smith, P.. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (1996c) Simulation and self-knowledge. In: Theories of Theories of Mind, ed. Carruthers, P. & Smith, P.. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2000) Phenomenal consciousness: A naturalistic theory. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002) The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:657719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P. (2006) The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2008a) Cartesian epistemology: Is the theory of the self-transparent mind innate? Journal of Consciousness Studies 15(4):2853.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2008b) Metacognition in animals: A skeptical look. Mind and Language 23(1):5889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2009) An architecture for dual reasoning. In: In two minds: Dual processes and beyond, ed. Evans, J. St. B. T. & Frankish, K., pp. 109–27. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. & Seyfarth, R. (2007) Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (1998) Magic words: How language augments human computation. In: Language and thought, ed. Carruthers, P. & Boucher, J.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. & Duncan, B. (1971) Cognitive dissonance as a function of self-esteem and logical inconsistency. Journal of Personality 18:354–63.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. (2007) Action mirroring and action interpretation: An alternative account. In: Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XXII, ed. Haggard, P., Rosetti, Y. & Kawato, M.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994) Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. Papermac.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003) Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. Harcourt.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. & Naccache, L. (2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79:137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D., Mangin, J., Poline, J. & Riviere, D. (2001) Cerebral mechanisms of word priming and unconscious repetition masking. Nature Neuroscience 4:752–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Sergent, C. & Changeux, J. (2003) A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 100:8520–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Penguin.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2000) Making tools for thinking. In: Metarepresentations, ed. Sperber, D.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (2000) On the origin of the human mind. In: Evolution and the human mind, ed. Carruthers, P. & Chamberlain, A.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eagly, A. & Chaiken, S. (1993) The psychology of attitudes. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Edwards, G. (1965) Post-hypnotic amnesia and post-hypnotic effect. British Journal of Psychiatry 111:316–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. (1982) The varieties of reference. Oxford University Press/Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. & Over, D. (1996) Rationality and reasoning. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Farrant, A., Boucher, J. & Blades, M. (1999) Metamemory in children with autism. Child Development 70:107–31.Google ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L. (1957) A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1992) A theory of the child's theory of mind. Cognition 44:283–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankish, K. (2004) Mind and supermind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. & Wolpert, D. (2000a) Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 355:1771–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. & Wolpert, D. (2000b) Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews 31:357–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. & Happé, F. (1999) Theory of mind and self-consciousness: What is it like to be autistic? Mind and Language 14:122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G. (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119:593609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12:493501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. (1995) Consciousness and the cerebral hemispheres. In: The cognitive neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, M.. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. (1998) The mind's past. California University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. (2000) Cerebral specialization and inter-hemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain 123:1293–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. & the ABC Research Group. (1999) Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1993) The psychology of folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:1528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (2006) Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, J. (1998) Some thoughts about the evolution of LADS, with special reference to TOM and SAM. In: Language and thought, ed. Carruthers, P. & Boucher, J.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1993) The illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1994) Minds, bodies, and persons: Young children's understanding of the self and others as reflected in imitation and theory of mind research. In: Self-awareness in animals and humans, ed. Parker, S., Mitchell, R. & Boccia, M.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1997) Words, thoughts, and theories. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, R. (1986) Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language 1:158–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, R. (1996) “Radical” simulationism. In: Theories of Theories of Mind, ed. Carruthers, P. & Smith, P.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Greenbaum, C. & Zemach, M. (1972) Role-playing and change of attitude toward the police after a campus riot: Effects of situational demand and justification. Human Relations 25:8799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grush, R. (2004) The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:377442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. R. (2001) Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98:5359–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. R. (2005) Can Rhesus monkeys discriminate between remembering and forgetting? In: The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-reflective Consciousness, ed. Terrace, H. & Metcalfe, J.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R., Zivin, A. & Murray, E. (2004) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminate between knowing and not knowing and collect information as needed before acting. Animal Cognition 7:239–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happé, F. (2003) Theory of mind and the self. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1001:134–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B. (2007) From nonhuman to human mind: What changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science 16:6064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B. & Tomasello, M. (2000) Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behavior 59:771–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2001) Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behavior 61:139–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (2002a) Checking our sources: The origins of trust in testimony. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science 33:315–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2002b) What do children learn from testimony? In: The cognitive basis of science, ed. Carruthers, P., Stich, S. & Siegal, M.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hurlburt, R. (1990) Sampling normal and schizophrenic inner experience. Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurlburt, R. (1993) Sampling inner experience in disturbed affect. Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurlburt, R. T. & Akhter, S. A. (2008) Unsymbolized thinking. Consciousness and Cognition 17:1364–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurlburt, R. T., Happé, F. & Frith, U. (1994) Sampling the form of inner experience in three adults with Asperger syndrome. Psychological Medicine 24:385–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (2006) Motor cognition. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2002) Maps of bounded rationality: A perspective on intuitive judgment and choice. Nobel laureate acceptance speech. Available at:http://nobelprize.org/economics/laureates/2002/kahneman-lecture.html.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1781/1929) The critique of pure reason, trans. Kemp Smith, N., Macmillan. (Original published in 1781.)Google Scholar
Kazak, S., Collis, G. & Lewis, V. (1997) Can young people with autism refer to knowledge states? Evidence from their understanding of “know” and “guess.” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38:10011009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A. (1993) How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review 100:609–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A. (1997) Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126:349–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koriat, A., Ma'ayan, H. & Nussinson, R. (2006) The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: Lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135(1):3669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornell, N., Son, L. & Terrace, H. (2007) Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychological Science 18:6471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. (1994) Image and brain. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreiman, G., Fried, I. & Koch, C. (2003) Single neuron correlates of subjective vision in the human medial temporal lobe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 99:8378–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A., Alon, W. & Lewis, T. (1972) Retrospective misattribution and task enjoyment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 8:493501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. & Polizzi, P. (1998) Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science 1:247–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1690/1961) An essay concerning human understanding, ed. Yolton, J., Dent and Sons. (Original published in 1690.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCloskey, M. (1983) Naive theories of motion. In: Mental models, ed. Gentner, D. & Stevens, A.. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J. & Shimamura, A., eds. (1994) Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T. O, ed. (1992) Metacognition: Core readings. Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. & Stich, S. (2003) Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. & Wilson, T. (1977) Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84:231–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K. & Baillargeon, R. (2005) Do 15-month-olds understand false beliefs? Science 5719:255–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K., Baillargeon, R. & Leslie, A. (2007) 15-month-old infants detect violations in pretend scenarios. Acta Psychologica 124:106–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Origgi, G. & Sperber, D. (2000) Evolution, communication, and the proper function of language. In: The evolution of the human mind, ed. Carruthers, P. & Chamberlain, A.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paulescu, E., Frith, D. & Frackowiak, R. (1993) The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 362:342–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S. & Rutter, M. (1998) Understanding intention in normal development and in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 16:337–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rey, G. (2008) (Even higher-order) intentionality without consciousness. Revue Internationale de Philosophie 62:5178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V. & Fogassi, L. (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research 3:131–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. & Hill, E. (2001) Action-monitoring and intention reporting in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42:317–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, B. L. & Smith, S. (1997) The retrieval of related information influences tip-of-the-tongue states. Journal of Memory and Language 36:6886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1992) The rediscovery of the mind. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988) From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheehan, P. & Orne, M. (1968) Some comments on the nature of post-hypnotic behavior. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 146:209–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shergill, S., Brammer, M., Fukuda, R., Bullmore, E., Amaro, E., Murray, R. & McGuire, P. (2002) Modulation of activity in temporal cortex during generation of inner speech. Human Brain Mapping 16:219–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shields, W., Smith, J. & Washburn, D. (1997) Uncertain responses by humans and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in a psychophysical same–different task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126:147–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shoemaker, S. (1996) The first-person perspective and other essays. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. (1996) The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 119:322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (2002) Two systems of reasoning. In: Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, ed. Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. & Kahneman, D.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D. (2005) Studies of uncertainty monitoring and metacognition in animals and humans. In: The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness, ed. Terrace, H. & Metcalfe, J.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Schull, J., Strote, J., McGee, K., Egnor, R. & Erb, L. (1995) The uncertain response in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124:391408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., Shields, W., Schull, J. & Washburn, D. (1997) The uncertain response in humans and animals. Cognition 62:7597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., Shields, W. & Washburn, D. (2003) The comparative psychology of uncertainty monitoring and metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26:317–39; discussion pp. 339–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Son, L. & Kornell, N. (2005) Meta-confidence judgments in rhesus macaques: Explicit versus implicit mechanisms. In: The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness, ed. Terrace, H. & Metcalfe, J.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Song, H. & Baillargeon, R.(forthcoming) Infants' reasoning about others' false perceptions. Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Song, H., Onishi, K., Baillargeon, R. & Fisher, C.(forthcoming) Can an agent's false belief be corrected through an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants. Cognition.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2008) Does the mirror neuron system and its impairment explain human imitation and autism? In: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition, ed. Pineda, J.. Humana Press.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Senju, A. & Csibra, G. (2007) Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science 18:587–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1995) Relevance: Communication and cognition, 2nd edition. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. (1999) Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, S. (2001) Separate modifiability, mental modules, and the use of pure and composite measures to reveal them. Acta Psychologica 106:147246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Surian, L., Caldi, S. & Sperber, D. (2007) Attribution of beliefs by 13-month old infants. Psychological Science 18:580–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrace, H. & Metcalfe, J., eds. (2005) The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. (2003a) Chimpanzees understand psychological states – the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:153–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. (2003b) Chimpanzees versus humans: It's not that simple. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:239–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Washburn, D., Smith, J. & Shields, W. (2006) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) immediately generalize the uncertain response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 32:185–89.Google ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. (2002) The illusion of conscious will. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegner, D. & Wheatley, T. (1999) Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of the will. American Psychologist 54:480–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L., Elliot, J. & Darlington, C. (1971) Preliminary observations of tickling oneself. Nature 230:598–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. (1990) The child's theory of mind. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H., Cross, D. & Watson, J. (2001) Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72:655–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G. & Petty, R. (1980) The effects of overt head movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 1:219–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicklund, R. & Brehm, J. (1976) Perspectives on cognitive dissonance. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. (2002) Strangers to ourselves. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. & Ghahramani, Z. (2000) Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience 3:1212–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, D. & Kawato, M. (1998) Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks 11:1317–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed