No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
How an interdisciplinary study of societies can develop a comprehensive understanding of the function of deceptive behavior
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 03 April 2025
Abstract
Moffett presents a robust proposal for a comparative study of societies as the basis for studying the human condition and behavior. This theoretical framework has implications for the study of deceptive behavior. I discuss how this framework might describe the adaptation of deceptive behavior within human societies and shed light on the dynamics of collaborative deceptive behavior through interpersonal commitment.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Allen, M. W., Bettinger, R. L., Codding, B. F., Jones, T. L., & Schwitalla, A. W. (2016). Resource scarcity drives lethal aggression among prehistoric hunter-gatherers in central California. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(43), 12120–12125.Google ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belmi, P., Barragan, R. C., Neale, M. A., & Cohen, G. L. (2015). Threats to social identity can trigger social deviance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(4), 467–484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorrough, A. R., Köbis, N., Irlenbusch, B., Shalvi, S., & Glöckner, A. (2023). Conditional bribery: Insights from incentivized experiments across 18 nations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(18), e2209731120.Google ScholarPubMed
Durkheim, E. (1912). The elementary forms of the religious life. George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Factor, R., Kawachi, I., & Williams, D. R. (2011). Understanding high-risk behavior among non-dominant minorities: A social resistance framework. Social Science & Medicine, 73(9), 1292–1301.Google ScholarPubMed
Gächter, S., & Schulz, J. F. (2016). Intrinsic honesty and the prevalence of rule violations across societies. Nature, 531(7595), 496–499.Google ScholarPubMed
Galak, J., & Critcher, C. R. (2023). Who sees which political falsehoods as more acceptable and why: A new look at in-group loyalty and trustworthiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 124(3), 593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holan, A. D. (2015). All politicians lie. Some lie more than others. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2015/12/13/opinion/campaign-stops/all-politicians-lie-some-lie-more-than-others.htmlGoogle Scholar
Karg, S. T., Kim, M., Mitkidis, P., & Young, L. (2023). Collaborative cheating in hierarchical teams: Effects of incentive structure and leader behavior on subordinate behavior and perceptions of leaders. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(8), 1166–1183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, L., & Jackson, A. L. (2013). Cooperation creates selection for tactical deception. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1762), 20130699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mearsheimer, J. J. (2011). Why leaders lie: The truth about lying in international politics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Michael, J., & Sebanz, N. (2016). The sense of commitment: A minimal approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 162497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitkidis, P., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Andersen, M., & Lienard, P. (2013). Collective-goal ascription increases cooperation in humans. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e64776.Google ScholarPubMed
Mitkidis, P., Perkovic, S., Nichols, A., Elbæk, C. T., Gerlach, P., & Ariely, D. (2023). Morality in minimally deceptive environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 30(1), 48–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Pascual-Ezama, D., Fosgaard, T. R., Cardenas, J. C., Kujal, P., Veszteg, R., de Liaño, B. G. G., … Branas-Garza, P. (2015). Context-dependent cheating: Experimental evidence from 16 countries. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 116, 379–386.Google Scholar
Sarkadi, Ş., Rutherford, A., McBurney, P., Parsons, S., & Rahwan, I. (2021). The evolution of deception. Royal Society Open Science, 8(9), 201032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thielmann, I., Böhm, R., & Hilbig, B. E. (2021). Buying unethical loyalty: A behavioral paradigm and empirical test. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(3), 363–370.Google Scholar
Tønnesen, M. H., Elbæk, C. T., Pfattheicher, S., & Mitkidis, P. (2024). Communication increases collaborative corruption. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 112, 104603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughan, L., & Finch, G. (2017) Libor scandal: The bankers who fixed the world's most important number. The Guardian. Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/business/2017/jan/18/libor-scandal-the-bankers-who-fixed-the-worlds-most-important-numberGoogle Scholar
Weisel, O., & Shalvi, S. (2015). The collaborative roots of corruption. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(34), 10651–10656.Google Scholar
Zickfeld, J. H., Karg, S. T. S., Engen, S. S., Gonzalez, A. S. R., Michael, J., & Mitkidis, P. (2024). Committed (dis) honesty: A systematic meta-analytic review of the divergent effects of social commitment to individuals or honesty oaths on dishonest behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 150(5), 586–620.Google ScholarPubMed
Target article
What is a society? Building an interdisciplinary perspective and why that's important
Related commentaries (24)
A nation by any other name: A failure to focus on function
Belonging to a community of moral values as a key criterion of society
Beyond biology: A sociological stance on what is society
Collective memories and understandings of human societies
Definitions and cultural dynamics in understanding “societies”
Do boundaries matter so much for societies?
Group identity without social interactions?
How an interdisciplinary study of societies can develop a comprehensive understanding of the function of deceptive behavior
Identity groups, perceived group continuity, and schism
Identity is probably too complicated to serve as a useful criterion for defining society
Multi-species societies
Philosophy or science of societies?
Psychological mechanisms for individual recognition- and anonymous-societies in humans and other animals
Revisiting the spaces of societies and the cooperation that sustains them
Societal inferences from the physical world
Societies have functions for individuals and collectives
Societies of the open ocean without territories
Societies, identities, and macrodemes
Society: An anthropological perspective
The family as the primary social group
Understanding the jaggedness in social complexity is more important
Vocalizations are ideal identity signals
What is a society in the case of multilevel societies?
Why societies are important and grow so large: Tribes, nations, and teams
Author response
A society as a clearly membered, enduring, territory-holding group