Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T07:23:00.941Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Goals reconfigure cognition by modulating predictive processes in the brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

Giovanni Pezzulo*
Affiliation:
Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, CNR, 00185 Roma, Italy. [email protected]://www.istc.cnr.it/people/giovanni-pezzulo

Abstract

I applaud Huang & Bargh's (H&B's) theory that places goals at the center of cognition, and I discuss two ingredients missing from that theory. First, I argue that the brains of organisms much simpler than those of humans are already configured for goal achievement in situated interactions. Second, I propose a mechanistic view of the “reconfiguration principle” that links the theory with current views in computational neuroscience.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bar, M. (2007) The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11:280–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. (2010) Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annual Review of Neuroscience 33:269–98.Google Scholar
Dayan, P. & Balleine, B. W. (2002) Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron 36:285–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18:193222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, H. & Friston, K. J. (2010) Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4:215.Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 360:815–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2010) The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11:127–38.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S.-J. & Wolpert, D. M. (2000) Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 355:1771–88.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. (1997) What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20:155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G. & Prinz, W. (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(5):849–78.Google Scholar
Memelink, J. & Hommel, B. (2013) Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychology Research 77:249–59.Google Scholar
Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24:167202.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montague, P. R. & King-Casas, B. (2007) Efficient statistics, common currencies and the problem of reward-harvesting. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11:514–19.Google Scholar
Mysore, S. P. & Knudsen, E. I. (2011) The role of a midbrain network in competitive stimulus selection. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 21:653–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzulo, G. (2008) Coordinating with the future: The anticipatory nature of representation. Minds and Machine 18:179–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pezzulo, G. (2013). Why do you fear the Bogeyman? An embodied predictive coding model of perceptual inference. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. Available at: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758%2Fs13415-013-0227-x Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. (2011) Grounding procedural and declarative knowledge in sensorimotor anticipation. Mind & Language 26:78114.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. (2012) An active inference view of cognitive control. Frontiers in Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 3:478. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00478.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Baldassarre, G., Butz, M. V., Castelfranchi, C. & Hoffmann, J. (2007) An analysis of the ideomotor principle and TOTE. In: Proceedings of the Third Workshop on Anticipatory Behavior in Adaptive Learning Systems (ABiALS 2006), Rome, Italy, ed. Butz, M. V., Sigaud, O., Pezzulo, G. & Baldassarre, G., pp. 7393. Springer-Verlag Berlin, Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. & Castelfranchi, C. (2007) The symbol detachment problem. Cognitive Processing 8(2):115–31.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. & Castelfranchi, C. (2009) Thinking as the control of imagination: A conceptual framework for goal-directed systems. Psychology Research 73:559–77.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F. & Chersi, F. (2013) The mixed instrumental controller: Using value of information to combine habitual choice and mental simulation. Frontiers in Cognition 4:92.Google Scholar
Proffitt, D. R. (2006) Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science 1:110–22.Google Scholar
Sharot, T., Korn, C. W. & Dolan, R. J. (2011) How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality. Nature Neuroscience 14:1475–79.Google Scholar
Solway, A. & Botvinick, M. M. (2012) Goal-directed decision making as probabilistic inference: A computational framework and potential neural correlates. Psychology Review 119:120–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verschure, P. F. M. J. (2012) Distributed adaptive control: A theory of the mind, brain, body nexus. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures 1:5572.Google Scholar