Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T06:12:47.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From perceived control to self-control, the importance of cognitive and emotional resources

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2017

Eyal Carmel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University, Beer Sheva 84105, Israel. [email protected]@bgu.ac.ilhttp://www.bgu.ac.il/~dleiser/
David Leiser
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University, Beer Sheva 84105, Israel. [email protected]@bgu.ac.ilhttp://www.bgu.ac.il/~dleiser/

Abstract

Pepper & Nettle (P&N) suggest that the poor present a “contextually appropriate response” to a perceived limited control and to a short life expectancy. We argue that differences in health, behavior, or impaired economic decisions are better explained by self-control. We discuss the implications of the differences between these perspectives and present supporting findings from two intervention studies with marginalized populations.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bernheim, B. D., Ray, D. & Yeltekin, Ş. (2015) Poverty and self-control. Econometrica 83(5):1877–911.Google Scholar
Carmel, E., Leiser, D. & Spivak, A. (submitted) Down and out. The effect of scarcity on financial capabilities among graduates of a financial intervention program.Google Scholar
Duflo, E. (2006) Poor but rational? In: Understanding poverty, ed. Banerjee, A., Mookherjee, D. & Benabou, R., pp. 367–78. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelstein, A. (2013) Evaluating The Efficacy Of The “Second Chance” Program In Preventing Recidivism Among Young Immigrant Offenders Over Time [Hebrew]. A research report for Yedidim for Youth and Society. Available at: http://www.moia.gov.il/Hebrew/Subjects/welfare/Documents/SikuimResearch_2013.pdf.Google Scholar
Loibl, C. (2017) Living in poverty: Understanding the financial behaviour of vulnerable groups. In: Economic psychology: The science of economic mental life and behaviour, ed. Ranyard, R., pp. pp. 421–34. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ludwig, J., Duncan, G. J., Gennetian, L. A., Katz, L. F., Kessler, R. C., Kling, J. R. & Sanbonmatsu, L. (2012) Neighborhood effects on the long-term well-being of low-income adults. Science 337(6101):1505–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E. & Zhao, J. (2013) Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science 341(6149):976–80.Google Scholar
Mullainathan, S. & Shafir, E. (2013) Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. Times Books, Macmillan and Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S. & Shafir, E. (2012) Some consequences of having too little. Science 338(6107):682–85.Google Scholar
Spivak, A. & Leiser, D. (2016) Economic analysis of the “second chance” program. Report presented to the president of the State of Israel. Yedidim for Youth and Society. Available at: http://yedidim-israel.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/06/cost-benefit-analysis-executive-summary.pdf.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D. (2013) Psychology: The poor's poor mental power. Science 341(6149):969–70.Google Scholar