No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The emergence of mirror-like response properties from domain-general principles in vision and audition
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 29 April 2014
Abstract
Like Cook et al., we suggest that mirror neurons are a fascinating product of cross-modal learning. As predicted by an associative account, responses in motor regions are observed for novel and/or abstract visual stimuli such as point-light and android movements. Domain-specific mirror responses also emerge as a function of audiomotor expertise that is slowly acquired over years of intensive training.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014
References
Barresi, J. & Moore, C. (1996) Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
19(1):107–54.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Bretherton, I. & Snyder, L. (1988) From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Caggiano, V, Fogassi, L, Rizzolatti, G., Pomper, J. K., Thier, P., Giese, M. A. & Casile, A. (2011) View-based encoding of actions in mirror neurons of area F5 in macaque premotor cortex. Current Biology
21(2):144–48.Google Scholar
Cross, E. S., Liepelt, R., Hamilton, A. F. de C., Parkinson, J., Ramsey, R., Stadler, W. & Prinz, W. (2012) Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network. Human Brain Mapping
33(9):2238–54.Google Scholar
Dick, F., Lee, H. L., Nusbaum, H. & Price, C. J. (2011) Auditory-motor expertise alters “speech selectivity” in professional musicians and actors. Cerebral Cortex
21(4):938–48.Google Scholar
Dick, F., Saygin, A. P., Galati, G., Pitzalis, S., Bentrovato, S., D'Amico, S., Wilson, S., Bates, E. & Pizzamiglio, L. (2007) What is involved and what is necessary for complex linguistic and non-linguistic auditory processing: Evidence from fMRI and lesion data. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
19(5):799–816.Google Scholar
Dick, F., Saygin, A. P., Moineau, S., Aydelott, J. & Bates, E. (2004) Language in an embodied brain: The role of animal models. Cortex
40:226–27.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K. (1996) Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G. & Fogassi, L. (2003) Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience
17(8):1703–14.Google Scholar
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B. & Keysers, C. (2007) The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage
35(4):1674–84.Google Scholar
Gilaie-Dotan, S., Kanai, R., Bahrami, B., Rees, G. & Saygin, A. P. (2013) Structural neural correlates of biological motion detection ability. Neuropsychologia
51(3):457–63.Google Scholar
Nelissen, K., Luppino, G., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G. & Orban, G. A. (2005) Observing others: Multiple action representation in the frontal lobe. Science
310(5746):332–36.Google Scholar
Oberman, L. M., McCleery, J. P., Ramachandran, V. S. & Pineda, J. A. (2007) EEG evidence for mirror neuron activity during the observation of human and robot actions: Toward an analysis of the human qualities of interactive robots. Neurocomputing
70:2194–203.Google Scholar
Saygin, A. P., Chaminade, T., Ishiguro, H., Driver, J. & Frith, C. (2012) The thing that should not be: Predictive coding and the uncanny valley in perceiving human and humanoid robot actions. Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience
7(4):413–22.Google Scholar
Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., Wilson, S. M., Dronkers, N. F. & Bates, E. (2003) Neural resources for processing language and environmental sounds: Evidence from aphasia. Brain
126(4): 928–45.Google Scholar
Saygin, A. P., Wilson, S. M., Hagler, D. J. Jr., Bates, E. & Sereno, M. I. (2004b) Point-light biological motion perception activates human premotor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience
24:6181–88.Google Scholar
Urgen, B. A., Plank, M., Ishiguro, H., Poizner, H. & Saygin, A. P. (2013) EEG Theta and Mu oscillations during perception of human and robot actions. Frontiers in Neurorobotics
7:19. doi:10.3389/fnbot.2013.00019.Google Scholar
Vangeneugden, J., Vancleef, K., Jaeggli, T., VanGool, L. & Vogels, R. (2009) Discrimination of locomotion direction in impoverished displays of walkers by macaque monkeys. Journal of Vision
10(4):22.1–19.Google Scholar
van Kemenade, B. M., Muggleton, N., Walsh, V. & Saygin, A. P. (2012) Effects of TMS over premotor and superior temporal cortices on biological motion perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
24(4):896–904.Google Scholar
Westermann, G. & Miranda, E. R. (2002) Modelling the development of mirror neurons for auditory-motor integration. Journal of New Music Research
31(4):367–75.Google Scholar
Westermann, G. & Miranda, E. R. (2004) A new model of sensorimotor coupling in the development of speech. Brain and Language
89:393–400.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. M., Saygin, A. P., Sereno, M. I. & Iacoboni, M. (2004) Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature Neuroscience
7:701–702.Google Scholar
Target article
The emergence of mirror-like response properties from domain-general principles in vision and audition
Related commentaries (1)
Mirror neurons: From origin to function