Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:17:29.808Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A developmental perspective on action and social cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

Sheila Krogh-Jespersen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637. [email protected]@[email protected]://woodwardlab.uchicago.edu/Home.html
Courtney Filippi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637. [email protected]@[email protected]://woodwardlab.uchicago.edu/Home.html
Amanda L. Woodward
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637. [email protected]@[email protected]://woodwardlab.uchicago.edu/Home.html

Abstract

The target article argues that developmental processes are key to understanding the mirror neuron system, yet neglects several bodies of developmental research that are informative for doing so. Infants' actions and action understanding are structured by goals, and the former lends structure to the latter. Evaluating the origins and functions of mirror neurons depends on integrating investigations of neural, social-cognitive and motor development.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brandone, A. C. & Wellman, H. M. (2009) You can't always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychological Science 20:8591. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02246.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brune, C. W. & Woodward, A. L. (2007) Social cognition and social responsiveness in 10-month-old infants. Journal of Cognition and Development 8:133–58. doi:10.1080/15248370701202356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannon, E. N., Fox, N., Vanderwert, R. E., Woodward, A. L. & Ferrari, P. F. (under review) The mirror neuron system in infant development: Relations between emerging motor skill and event-related desynchronization in EEG.Google Scholar
Cannon, E. N. & Woodward, A. L. (2012) Infants generate goal-based action predictions. Developmental Science 15:292–98. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01127.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, E. N., Woodward, A. L., Gredebäck, G., von Hofsten, C. & Turek, C. (2012) Action production influences 12-month-old infants' attention to others' actions. Developmental Science 15:3542. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01095.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claxton, L. J., Keen, R. & McCarty, M. E. (2003) Evidence of motor planning in infant reaching behavior. Psychological Science 14:354–56. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.24421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerson, S. & Woodward, A. L. (2012) A claw is like my hand: Comparison supports goal analysis in infants. Cognition 122:181–92. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerson, S. A. & Woodward, A. L. (2014) Learning from their own actions: The unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. Child Development 85:264–77. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gredebäck, G., Stasiewicz, D., Falck-Ytter, T., Rosander, K. & von Hofsten, C. (2009) Action type and goal type modulate goal-directed gaze shifts in 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 45:1190–94. doi:10.1037/a0015667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Hallinan, E. V. & Woodward, A. (2008) Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others' goals. Developmental Science 11:487–94. doi:10.1111/j.14677687.2008.00694.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanakogi, Y. & Itakura, S. (2010) The link between perception and action in early infancy: From the viewpoint of the direct-matching hypothesis. Japanese Psychological Research 52:121–31. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5884.2010.00429.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanakogi, Y. & Itakura, S. (2011) Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. Nature Communications 2:341. doi:10.1038/ncomms1342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krogh-Jespersen, S. & Woodward, A. L. (under review) Making smart social judgments takes time: Infants' recruitment of goal information when generating action predictions.Google Scholar
Libertus, K. & Needham, A. (2010) Teach to reach: The effects of active versus passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Research 50:2750–57. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loucks, J. & Sommerville, J. A. (2012) The role of motor experience in understanding action function: The case of the precision grasp. Child Development 83:801809. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01735.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y. & Johnson, S. C. (2009) Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. Developmental Science 12:142–49. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00741.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, P. J., Saby, J. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2013) Infant brain responses to object weight: Exploring goal-directed actions and self-experience. Infancy.18:942–60. doi:10.1111/infa.12012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, P. J., Young, T. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011) Neural correlates of action observation and execution in 14-month-old infants: An event-related EEG desynchronization study. Developmental Science 14:474–80. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00991.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995) Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactments of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 31(5):838–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyström, P., Ljunghammar, T., Rosander, K. & von Hofsten, C. (2011) Using mu-rhythm perturbations to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. Developmental Science 14(2):327–35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00979.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., van Elk, M. & Bekkering, H. (2012) How learning to shake a rattle affects 8-month-old infants' perception of the rattle's sound: Electrophysiological evidence for action-effect binding in infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 2:9096. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, D. (1991) Human motor control. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sodian, B. & Thoermer, C. (2004) Infants' understanding of looking, pointing, and reaching as cues to goal-directed action. Journal of Cognition and Development 5:289316. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0503_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommerville, J. A., Hildebrand, E. A. & Crane, C. C. (2008) Experience matters: The impact of doing versus watching on infants' subsequent perception of tool use events. Developmental Psychology 44:1249–56. doi:10.1037/a0012296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerville, J. A. & Woodward, A. L. (2005) Pulling out the intentional structure of human action: The relation between action production and processing in infancy, Cognition 95:130. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.12.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L. & Needham, A. (2005) Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others actions. Cognition 96:B1B11. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Southgate, V., Johnson, M. H., Osborne, T. & Csibra, G. (2009) Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants. Biology Letters 5(6):769–72. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., Corbetta, D. & Spencer, J. P. (1996) Development of reaching during the first year: Role of movement speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22:1059–75. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.22.5.1059.Google ScholarPubMed
Van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C. & Bekkering, H. (2008) You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage 43:808–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Hofsten, C. (1980) Predictive reaching for moving objects by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 30:369–82. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(80)90043-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Hofsten, C. (2004) An action perspective on motor development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:266–72. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Hofsten, C. & Ronnqvist, L. (1988) Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14:610–21. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.14.4.610.Google ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. L. (1998) Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition 69:134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed