Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T18:44:56.022Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can the shared circuits model (SCM) explain joint attention or perception of discrete emotions?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2008

Bhismadev Chakrabarti
Affiliation:
Autism Research Centre, Cambridge CB2 8AH, United Kingdom. [email protected]://people.pwf.cam.ac.uk/[email protected]://www.autismresearchcentre.com
Simon Baron-Cohen
Affiliation:
Autism Research Centre, Cambridge CB2 8AH, United Kingdom. [email protected]://people.pwf.cam.ac.uk/[email protected]://www.autismresearchcentre.com

Abstract

The shared circuits model (SCM) is a bold attempt to explain how humans make sense of action, at different levels. In this commentary we single out five concerns: (1) the lack of a developmental account, (2) the absence of double-dissociation evidence, (3) the neglect of joint attention and joint action, (4) the inability to explain discrete emotion perception, and (5) the lack of predictive power or testability of the model. We conclude that Hurley's model requires further work before it could be seen as an improvement over earlier models.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995a) Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press/Bradford Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995b) The eye direction detector (EDD) and the shared attention mechanism (SAM): Two cases for evolutionary psychology. In: Joint attention: Its role in development, ed. Moore, C. & Dunham, P., pp. 4160. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. & Wheelwright, S. (2004) The empathy quotient (EQ): An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:163–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, J. & Perschardt, K. (2003) Empathy: A unitary circuit or a set of dissociable neuro-cognitive systems? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(1):2728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J., Jones, L., Clark, F. & Smith, M. (1997) The psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology 34:192–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M. & Striano, T. (2005) Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy 8(3):253–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chakrabarti, B. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006) Empathizing: Neurocognitive developmental mechanisms and individual differences. Progress in Brain Research 156:406–17. (Special issue on “Understanding Emotions”.)Google ScholarPubMed
Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006) Empathising with basic emotions: Common and discrete neural substrates. Social Neuroscience 1(3–4):364–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (2001) Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron 32:969–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T.-W., Josephs, O., Dolan, R. J. & Critchley, H. D. (2006) Imitating expressions: Emotion-specific neural substrates in facial mimicry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1(2):122–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. & Moore, M. (1983b) Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development 54(3):702709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. & Moore, M. (1989) Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology 25(6):954–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S. & Rutter, M. (1995) To what extent do children with autism understand desires? Development and Psychopathology 7:151–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. (2005) Imitation and moral development. In: Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science, vol. 2, ed. Hurley, S. & Chater, N., pp. 267–82. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Richell, M., Newman, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. & Blair, (2003) Theory of mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals read the “language of the eyes”? Neuropsychologia 41:523–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, H. & Lollis, S. (1987) Communication within infant social games. Developmental Psychology 23(2):241–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scaife, M. & Bruner, J. (1975) The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature 253:265–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5):675–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed