Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T18:03:28.446Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bonding system in nonhuman primates and biological roots of musicality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Yuko Hattori*
Affiliation:
Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Center for International Collaboration and Advanced Studies in Primatology (CICASP), Kanrin, Inuyama, Aichi484-8506, Japan. [email protected]; http://www.cicasp.pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp/people/yuko-hattori

Abstract

Comparative studies of primates indicate that humans have evolved unique motivations and cognitive skills for sharing emotions, experiences, and collaborative actions. Given the characteristics of music, the music and social bonding (MSB) hypothesis by Savage et al. fits this view. Within a cross-species approach, predispositions not observed in current communication system may contribute to a better understanding of the biological roots of human musicality.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., & Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends in the Cognitive Sciences, 14, 208215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Uniquely expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Elliott, R., Newman, J. L., Longe, O. A., & Deakin, J. F. W. (2003). Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex in financial reward in humans: A parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 303307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erikson, K. I., Boot, W. R., Basak, C., Neider, M. B., Prakesh, R. S., Voss, M. W., Graybiel, A. M., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., & Kramer, A. F. (2010). Striatal volume predicts level of video game skill acquisition. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 4557.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. (1993). Primate juvenile and primate play. In Pereira, M. E. & Fairbanks, L. A. (Eds.), Juvenile primates (pp. 182196). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. In Pattern of behavior (p. 335). The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Graham, K. L. (2011). Coevolutionary relationship between striatum size and social play in nonhuman primates. American Journal of Primatology, 73(4), 314322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harcourt, A. H., & de Waal, F. B. M. (1992). Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y. (in press). Behavioral coordination and synchronization in non-human primates. In Anderson, J.R. & Kuroshima, H. (Eds.), Comparative cognition: Commonalities and diversity. Springer.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2015). Distractor effect of auditory rhythms on self-paced tapping in chimpanzees and humans. PLoS ONE, 10, e0130682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hattori, Y., & Tomonaga, M. (2020a). Rhythmic swaying induced by sound in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(2), 936942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hattori, Y., & Tomonaga, M. (in press). Reply to Bertolo et al.: Rhythmic swaying in chimpanzees has implications for understanding the biological roots of human music and dance. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2013). Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Scientific Report, 3, 1566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2008). Imitation recognition in great apes. Current Biology, 18(7), R288R290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, M. N., & Mitani, J. C. (2005). Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 275331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E. (2018). Not just for fun! Social play as a springboard for adult social competence in human and non-human primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72(6), 90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Burghardt, G. M., Smuts, B., Cordoni, G., Dall'Olio, S., Fouts, H. N., Řeháková-Petrů, M., Siviy, S. M., & Pellis, S. M. (2016a). Rough- and tumble play as a window on animal communication. Biological Reviews, 91, 311327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Cordoni, G., Demuru, E., & Bekoff, M. (2016b). Fair play and its connection with social tolerance, reciprocity and the ethology of peace. Behaviour, 153, 11951216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., Borelli, E., VOsalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2005). Macaques (Macaca nemestrina) recognize when they are being imitated. Biology Letters, 1(2), 219222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science, 325(5942), 880883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiura, H. (1998). Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese macaques. Animal Behaviour, 55, 673687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. Springer.Google Scholar
Van Schaik, CP. (2016). The primate origins of human nature. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Vink, M., Kahn, R. S., Raemaekers, M., van den Heuvel, M., Boersma, M., & Ramsey, N. F. (2005). Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 336344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willems, E. P., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Collective action and the intensity of between-group competition in non-human primates. Behavioral Ecology, 26(2), 625631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarco, W., Merchant, H., Prado, L., & Mendez, J. C. (2009). Subsecond timing in primates: Comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 31913202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed