We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Darby, C. L., and Riopelle, A. J.Observational learning in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 94–98, 1959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davenport, R. K., and Rogers, C. M.Intermodal equivalence of stimuli in apes. Science, 168, 279–280, 1970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, H., and Bernheim, J. W.Retention of fixed-interval performance in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 839–841, 1963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, H., and Steinman, F.Retention of runway performance as a function of proactive interference. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 834–838, 1963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, D. O., and Thompson, W. R. The social significance of animal studies. In Lindzey, G., (ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology, vol. 2, Reading, Mass., Addison-Wesley, 1954.Google Scholar
Michotte, A.La perception de la causalite. Louvain, Institut Supérieur de Philosophie, 1946.Google Scholar