Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T23:17:17.514Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Alternative paradigmatic hypotheses cannot be fairly evaluated from within one's own paradigmatic assumptions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 March 2004

Steven Lehar*
Affiliation:
Peli Lab, Schepens Eye Research Institute, Boston, MA02114-2500http://cns-alumni.bu.edu/~slehar

Abstract:

To avoid endless and futile debate, critics of an alternative paradigmatic hypothesis cannot simply state their own paradigmatic assumptions as if they were plain fact while dismissing those of the opposition as self-evidently absurd, because it is exactly those initial assumptions that are brought into question by the paradigmatic proposal. Perceived incredibility is no valid ground for rejection of a paradigm whose alternatives are at least equally incredible, and arguably more so.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelson, E. H. (1993) Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. Science 262:2042–44. [JAS]Google Scholar
Adelson, E. H. (2000) Lightness perception and lightness illusions. In: The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd edition, ed. Gazzaniga, M. S. MIT Press. [rSL, JH]Google Scholar
Aldrich, V. C. (1979) Objective sense-data. The Personalist 60:3642. [EW]Google Scholar
Amit, D. J. (1994) The Hebbian paradigm reintegrated: Local reverberations as internal representations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:617–26. [WD]Google Scholar
Andrews, D. P. (1964) Error-correcting perceptual mechanisms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 16:102–15. [HER]Google Scholar
Angelucci, A., Levitt, J. B., Walton, E. J., Hupe, J. M., Bullier, J. & Lund, J. S. (2002) Circuits for local and global signal integration in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 22:8633–46. [NPM]Google Scholar
Anstis, S. & Howard, I. (1978) A Craik-O’Brien-Cornsweet illusion for visual depth. Vision Research 18:213–17. [aSL]Google Scholar
Aristotle (1976) De anima (On the soul). Arno Press. [JAS]Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1969) Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye. University of California Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Attneave, F. (1971) Multistability in perception. Scientific American 225:142–51. [aSL]Google Scholar
Attneave, F. (1982) Prägnanz and soap bubble systems: A theoretical exploration. In: Organization and representation in perception, ed. Beck, J. Erlbaum. [aSL, RL, SM]Google Scholar
Ayer, A. J. (1957) The problem of knowledge. Penguin Books. [EW]Google Scholar
Barlow, H., Blakemore, C. & Pettigrew, J. (1967) The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination. Journal of Physiology 193:327–42. [aSL]Google Scholar
Barrow, H. G. & Tenenbaum, J. M. (1981) Interpreting line drawings as threedimensional surfaces. Artificial Intelligence 17:75116. [aSL]Google Scholar
Bisiach, E., Capitani, E., Luzatti, C. & Perani, D. (1981) Brain and conscious representation of outside reality. Neuropsychologia 19:543–52. [aSL]Google Scholar
Bisiach, E. & Luzatti, C. (1978) Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex 14:129–33. [aSL]Google Scholar
Blakemore, C. & Tobin, E. A. (1972) Lateral inhibition between orientation detectors in the cat's visual cortex. Experimental Brain Research 15:439–40. [NPM]Google Scholar
Blank, A. A. (1958) Analysis of experiments in binocular space perception. Journal of the Optical Society of America 48:911–25. [aSL]Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, W. (1913) Untersuchungen Über die Scheinbare Grösse im Sehraume. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 65:241404. [aSL]Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (1978) Mind-brain puzzle versus mind-physical world identity. (Commentary on R. Puccetti & R. W. Dykes, Sensory cortex and the mindbrain problem.) Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:348–49. [DAB]Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. & Freeman, R. P. J. (1993) Discriminative measurement of feature integration in object recognition. Acta Psychologica 84:116. [DAB]Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1933) The physical dimensions of consciousness. Century. [aSL]Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1942) Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century. [RL]Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1950) A history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [RL]Google Scholar
Bosking, W. H., Zhang, Y., Schofield, B. & Fitzpatrick, D. (1997) Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 17:2112–27. [NPM]Google Scholar
Bressan, P. (1993) Neon colour spreading with and without its figural prerequisites. Perception 22:353–61. [aSL]Google Scholar
Broad, C. D. (1925) The mind and its place in nature. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [aSL]Google Scholar
Brookes, A. & Stevens, K. (1989) The analogy between stereo depth and brightness. Perception 18:601–14. [aSL]Google Scholar
Bruce, V. & Green, P. (1987) Visual perception: Physiology, psychology, and ecology. Erlbaum. [aSL]Google Scholar
Carman, G. J. & Welch, L. (1992) Three-dimensional illusory contours and surfaces. Nature 360:585–87. [aSL]Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. J. (1995) Facing up to the problems of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 2:200–19. Reprinted in: Toward a science of consciousness II: The second Tucson discussions and debates (1996), ed. Hameroff, S. R., Kaszniak, A. W. & Scott, A. C. MIT Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Charnwood, J. R. B. (1951) Essay on binocular vision. Halton Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology. [CRF]Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1984) Matter and consciousness: A contemporary introduction to the philosophy of mind. MIT Press. [aSL, WD]Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1993) Sensory qualities. Oxford University Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1998) Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press. [CRF]Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2000) A theory of sentience. Oxford University Press. [DLl]Google Scholar
Collett, T. (1985) Extrapolating and interpolating surfaces in depth. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 224:4356. [aSL]Google Scholar
Coren, S., Ward, L. M. & Enns, J. J. (1994) Sensation and perception. Harcourt Brace. [aSL]Google Scholar
Cornsweet, T. N. (1970) Visual perception. Academic Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Crick, F. (1994) The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search for the soul. Scribners. [aSL]Google Scholar
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (1990) Toward a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences 2:263–75. [aSL,SM]Google Scholar
Cronly-Dillon, J. R. & Gregory, R. L., eds. (1991) Vision and visual dysfunction, vol. 2: Evolution of the eye and visual system. CRC Press. [DDH]Google Scholar
Cutting, J. E. & Vishton, P. M. (1995) Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The interaction, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In: Perception of space and motion, ed. Epstein, W. & Rogers, S. Academic Press. [JH]Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1970) Mental events. Oxford University Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1981) Two approaches to mental images. In: Imagery, ed. Block, N. MIT Press. [arSL]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown. [aSL]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1992) “Filling in” versus finding out: A ubiquitous confusion in cognitive science. In: Cognition: Conceptual and methodological issues, ed. Pick, H. L. Jr., van den Broek, P. & Knill, D. C. American Psychological Association. [aSL]Google Scholar
Denny-Brown, D. & Chambers, R. A. (1958) The parietial lobe and behavior. Annual Research in Nervous and Mental Disease 36:35117. [aSL]Google Scholar
de Renzi, E. (1982) Disorders of space exploration and cognition. Wiley. [aSL]Google Scholar
Deubel, H. et al. (1998) Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy. Vision Research 38:3147–59. [PUT]Google Scholar
Dobbins, A. C., Jeo, R. M., Fiser, J. & Allman, J. M. (1998) Distance modulation of neural activity in the visual cortex. Science 281:552–55. [HER]Google Scholar
Drake, D. (1920) The approach to critical realism. In Essays in critical realism: A co-operative study of the problem of knowledge, pp. 332, ed. Drake, D., Lovejoy, A. O., Pratt, J. B., Rogers, A. K., Santayana, G., Sellars, R. W. & Strong, C. A. Gordian Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Drake, D. (1925) Mind and its place in nature. Macmillan. [EW]Google Scholar
Drake, D., Lovejoy, A. O., Pratt, J. B., Rogers, A. K., Santayana, G., Sellars, R. W. & Strong, C. A. (1920) Essays in critical realism: A co-operative study of the problem of knowledge. Gordian Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Duch, W. (1997) Platonic model of mind as an approximation to neurodynamics. In: Brain-like computing and intelligent information systems, ed. Amari, S.-I. & Kasabov, N. Springer. [WD]Google Scholar
Duffy, C. & Wurtz, R. (1995) Response of monkey MST neurons to optic flow stimuli with shifted centers of motion. Journal of Neuroscience 15:5192–208. [CRF]Google Scholar
Duffy, C. & Wurtz, R. (1997a) Medial superior temporal area neurons respond to speed patterns in optic flow. Journal of Neuroscience 17:2839–51. [CRF]Google Scholar
Duffy, C. & Wurtz, R. (1997b) Planar directional contributions to optic flow responses in MST neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 77:782–96. [CRF]Google Scholar
Earle, D. C. (1998) On the roles of consciousness and representations in visual science. Commentary on Pessoa et al. 1998. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:757–58. [aSL]Google Scholar
Ebenholtz, S. M. (2001) Oculomotor systems and perception. Cambridge University Press. [CRF]Google Scholar
Eckhorn, R., Bauer, R., Jordan, W., Brosch, M., Kruse, W., Munk, M. & Reitboeck, J. (1988) Coherent oscillations: A mechanism of feature linking in the visual cortex? Biological Cybernetics 60:121–30. [aSL, SM]Google Scholar
Einstein, A. (1950) Essays in physics. Philosophical Library. [DDH]Google Scholar
Evans, G. (1982) The varieties of reference. Clarendon Press. [EW]Google Scholar
Farah, M. J. (1990) Visual agnosia. MIT Press. [NPM]Google Scholar
Fechner, G. T. (1860/1966) Elemente der Psychophysik. Breitkopf and Härtel. English edition, 1966: Elements of psychophysics, vol. 1, trans. Adler, H. E. Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [DL]Google Scholar
Feigl, H. (1958) The “mental” and the “physical.” University of Minnesota Press. [arSL]Google Scholar
Felisberti, F. & Derrington, A. M. (2001) Long-range interactions in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the New-World monkey, Callithrix jacchus. Visual Neuroscience 18:209–18. [NPM]Google Scholar
Foley, J. M. (1978) Primary distance perception. In: Handbook of sensory physiology, vol. 7: Perception, ed. Held, R., Leibowitz, H. W. & Tauber, H.-L. Springer Verlag. [aSL]Google Scholar
Forsee, A. (1963) Albert Einstein: Theoretical physicist. Macmillan. [DDH]Google Scholar
Foster, J. (2000) The nature of perception. Oxford University Press. [EW]Google Scholar
Fox, C. R. (1999) Special senses 3: The visual system. In: Neuroscience for rehabilitation, 2nd edition, ed. Cohen, H. Lippincott Williams & Williams. [CRF]Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Craighero, L., Fadiga, L.& Fogassi, L. (1999) Perception through action. PSYCHE 5, 1999; Online journal: http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v5/psyche-5-21-gallese.html [CRF]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1950) The perception of the visual world. Houghton Mifflin. [VR]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin. [RL, CRF]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1972) A theory of direct visual perception. In: The psychology of knowing, ed. Royce, J. R. & Rozeboom, W. W. Gordon & Breach. [aSL]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1977) The theory of affordances. In: Perceiving, acting and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology, ed. Shaw, R. & Bransford, J. Erlbaum. [EW]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin. [CRF, aSL, RL, SM]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. & Crooks, L. E. (1938) A theoretical field-analysis of automobile driving. The American Journal of Psychology 51:453–71. [aSL]Google Scholar
Gilchrist, A. L. (1977) Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement. Science 195:185–87. [JAS]Google Scholar
Gillam, B. (1971) A depth processing theory of the Poggendorf illusion. Perception and Psychophysics 10:211–16. [aSL]Google Scholar
Gillam, B. (1980) Geometrical illusions. Scientific American 242:102–11. [aSL]Google Scholar
Gillam, B. (1998) Illusions at century's end. In: Handbook of perception and cognition, 2nd edition, ed. Hochberg, J. Academic Press. [HER]Google Scholar
Graham, C. H. (1965) Visual space perception. In: Vision and visual perception, ed. Graham, C. H. Wiley. [aSL]Google Scholar
Grealy, M. (2002) Closing gaps: Can the generalized intrinsic T-guide model provide a unified account of brain and behavior? Paper presented at the Seventh European Workshop on Ecological Psychology, Bendor Island, France, July 46, 2002. [CRF]Google Scholar
Green, M. & Odum, V. J. (1986) Correspondence matching in apparent motion: Evidence for three dimensional spatial representation. Science 233:1427–29. [aSL]Google Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1963) Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling. Nature 199:678–79. [aSL]Google Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1971) The intelligent eye. Weidenfeld & Nicolson. [SM]Google Scholar
Greve, D., Grossberg, S., Guenther, F. & Bullock, D. (1993) Neural representations for sensory-motor control, I: Head-centered 3-D target positions from opponent eye commands. Acta Psychologica 82:115–38. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1987) Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional form, color, and brightness perception: II. Binocular theory. Perception and Psychophysics 41:117–58. [SG, aSL]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1990) Neural FAÇADE: Visual representations of static and moving form-and-color-and-depth. Mind and Language 5 (Special Issue on Understanding Vision):411–56. [aSL]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1994) 3-D vision and figure-ground separation by visual cortex. Perception and Psychophysics 55:48120. [SG, aSL, NPM, VR]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1995) The attentive brain. American Scientist 83:483–49. [WD]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1997) Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional figure-ground perception of twodimensional pictures. Psychological Review 104:618–58. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1999a) How does the cerebral cortex work? Learning, attention, and grouping by the laminar circuits of visual cortex. Spatial Vision 12:163–86. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1999b) The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8:144. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (2000) How hallucinations may arise from brain mechanisms of learning, attention, and volition. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 6:583–92. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Howe, P. D. L. (2003) A laminar cortical model of stereopsis and three-dimensional surface perception. Vision Research. (in press). [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Kelly, F. (1999) Neural dynamics of binocular brightness perception. Vision Research 39:3796–816. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & McLoughlin, N. (1997) Cortical dynamics of 3-D surface perception: Binocular and half-occluded scenic images. Neural Networks 10:1583–605. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Mingolla, E. (1985) Neural dynamics of form perception: Boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. Psychological Review 92:173211. [aSL]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S., Mingolla, E. & Pack, C. (1999) A neural model of motion processing and visual navigation by cortical area MST. Cerebral Cortex 9:878–95. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S., Mingolla, E. & Ross, W. D. (1997) Visual brain and visual perception: How does the cortex do perceptual grouping? Trends in Neurosciences 20:106–11. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Pessoa, L. (1998) Texture segregation, surface representation, and figure-ground separation. Vision Research 38:2657–84. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Raizada, R. (2000) Contrast-sensitive perceptual grouping and object-based attention in the laminar circuits of primary visual cortex. Vision Research 40:1413–32. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Seitz, A. (2003) Laminar development of receptive fields, maps, and columns in visual cortex: The coordinating role of the subplate. Cerebral Cortex (in press). [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Swaminathan, G. (2003) A laminar cortical model for visual perception of slanted and curved 3D surfaces and 2D images: Development, attention, and bistability (submitted). [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S & Todoroviç, D. (1988) Neural dynamics of 1-D and 2-D brightness perception: A unified model of classical and recent phenomena. Perception and Psychophysics 43:241–77. [aSL]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. & Williamson, J. W. (2001) A neural model of how horizontal and interlaminar connections of visual cortex develop into adult circuits that carry out perceptual grouping and learning. Cerebral Cortex 11:3758. [SG]Google Scholar
Guenther, F., Bullock, D., Greve, D. & Grossberg, S. (1994) Neural representations for sensory-motor control, III: Learning a body-centered representation of 3-D target position. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 6:341–58. [SG]Google Scholar
Gunderson, K. (1971) Asymmetries and mind-body perplexities. Minnesota Studies in Philosophy of Science 4:273309. [KG]Google Scholar
Gunderson, K. (1984) Skinnerian privacy and Leibnizian privacy. (Invited commentary on aspects of B. F. Skinner's work with response from Skinner). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(4): Reprinted in: Canonical papers of B. F. Skinner, ed. Stephen Harnad, Cambridge University Press, 1988. [KG]Google Scholar
Harrison, S. (1989) A new visualization on the mind-brain problem: Naive realism transcended. In: The case for dualism, ed. Smythies, J. & Beloff, J. University of Virginia Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Hatfield, G. & Epstein, W. (1985) The status of minimum principle in the theoretical analysis of visual perception. Psychological Bulletin 97:155–86. [SM]Google Scholar
Heckenmuller, E. G. (1965) Stabilization of the retinal image: A review of method, effects, and theory. Psychological Bulletin 63:157–69. [aSL]Google Scholar
Heelan, P. A. (1983) Space perception and the philosophy of science. University of California Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Heilman, K. M. & Watson, R. T. (1977) The neglect syndrome – A unilateral defect of the orienting response. In: Lateralization in the nervous system, ed. Harnad, S., Doty, R. W., Goldstein, L., Jaynes, J. & Krauthamer, G. Academic Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T. & Valenstein, E. (1985) Neglect and related disorders. In: Clinical neuropsychology, ed. Heilman, K. M. & Valenstein, E. Oxford University Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Helmholtz, H. (1925) Physiological optics. Optical Society of America 3:318. [aSL]Google Scholar
Hillebrand, F. (1902) Theorie der Scheinbaren Grösse bei Binocularem Sehen. Denkschrift von acadamesche Wissenschaft, Wien 72:255307. [aSL]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1970) Attention, organization and consciousness. In: Attention: Contemporary theory and analysis, ed. Mostofsky, D. I. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [JH]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1978) Perception. Prentice Hall. [SM]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1998) Gestalt theory and its legacy: Organization in eye and brain, in attention and mental representation. In: Perception and cognition at century's end, ed. Hochberg, J. Academic Press. [JH]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (in press) Acts of perceptual inquiry: Parsing objects by diagnostic coupling and consequences. Acta Psychologica. [JH]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. & Brooks, V. (1960) The psychophysics of form: Reversible perspective drawings of spatial objects. The American Journal of Psychology 73:337–54. [aSL]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. & McAlister, E. (1953) A quantitative approach to “figural goodness.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 46:361–64. [JH]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. & Peterson, M. A. (1987) Piecemeal organization and cognitive components in object perception: Perceptually coupled responses to moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116:370–80. [JH]Google Scholar
Hoffman, D. D. (1998) Visual intelligence: How we create what we see. W. W. Norton. [aSL]Google Scholar
Hoffman, W. C. (1989) The visual cortex is a contact bundle. Applied Mathematics and Computation 32:137–67. [VR]Google Scholar
Howe, P. D. L. & Grossberg, S. (2001) Laminar cortical circuits for stereopsis and surface depth perception. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts Vol. 164, No. 17. [SG]Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1959) Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology 148:574–91. [aSL]Google Scholar
Humphrey, G. K. & Goodale, M. A. (1998) Probing unconscious visual processing with the McCollough Effect. Consciousness and Cognition 7:494519. [NPM]Google Scholar
Hurvich, L. & Jameson, D. (1957) An opponent-colors theory of color vision. Psychological Review 64:384404. [JH]Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1907/1997) Thing and space: Lectures of 1907. Kluwer Academic. [VR]Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1966) Zur Phänomenologie des inneren Zeitbewusstseins (Phenomenology of inner time consciousness). Martinus Nijhoff. [DLl]Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1974) Ding und Raum (Thing and space): Lectures of 1907. Martinus Nijhoff. [DLl]Google Scholar
Idesawa, M. (1991) Perception of 3-D illusory surface with binocular viewing. Japanese Journal of Applied Physics 30(4B):L751L754. [aSL]Google Scholar
Indow, T. (1991) A critical review of Luneberg's model with regard to global structure of visual space. Psychological Review 98:430–53. [aSL]Google Scholar
Intraub, H. (1997) The representation of visual scenes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1:217–21. [JH]Google Scholar
Intraub, H., Hoffman, J. E., Wetherhold, C. J. & Stoehs, S. (2001) Does direction of a planned eye movement affect boundary extension? Paper presented at the Forty-first Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Orlando, FL, November, 2001. [JH]Google Scholar
James, W. (1904) Does “consciousness” exist? In: Body and mind: Readings in philosophy, ed. Vesey, G. N. A. George Allen & Unwin. [MV]Google Scholar
Jameson, D. & Hurvich, L. (1967) The science of color appearance. Color Engineering 5:29. [JH]Google Scholar
Jones, H. E., Andolina, I. M., Oakely, N. M., Murphy, P. C. & Sillito, A. M. (2000) Spatial summation in lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex. Experimental Brain Research 135:279–84. [NPM]Google Scholar
Jones, H. E., Grieve, K. L., Wang, W. & Sillito, A. M. (2001) Surround suppression in primate V1. Journal of Neurophysiology 86:2011–28. [NPM]Google Scholar
Julesz, B. (1971) Foundations of Cyclopean perception. University of Chicago Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. (1979) Organization in vision. Praeger. [aSL,RL]Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. (1994) Gestalt theory has been misinterpreted, but also has had some real conceptual difficulties. Philosophical Psychology 7:149–62. [RL]Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. & Luccio, R. (1986) Die Doppeldeutigkeiten der Prägnanz. Gestalt Theory 8:99135. [RL]Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. & Luccio, R. (1990) The phenomenology of autonomous order formation in perception. In: Synergetics of cognition, ed. Haken, H. & Stadler, M. Springer. [RL]Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1781/1991) Critique of pure reason, ed. Vasilis, Politis Dent. [arSL]Google Scholar
Kapadia, M. K., Westheimer, G. & Gilbert, C. D. (2000) Spatial distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in visual perception. Journal of Neurophysiology 84:2048–62. [NPM]Google Scholar
Kassubek, J., Otte, M., Wolter, T., Greenlee, M. W., Mergner, T. & Lücking, C. H. (1999) Brain imaging in a patient with hemimicropsia. Neuropsychologia 37:1327–34. [HER]Google Scholar
Kaufman, L. (1974) Sight and mind. Oxford University Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kellman, P. J., Machado, L. J., Shipley, T. F. & Li, C. C. (1996) Three-dimensional determinants of object completion. Annual Review of Vision and Ophthalmology (ARVO) Abstracts 3133 37 (3):S685. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kellman, P. J. & Shipley, T. F. (1991) A theory of visual interpolation in object perception. Cognitive Psychology 23:141221. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kelly, F. & Grossberg, S. (2000) Neural dynamics of 3-D surface perception: Figure-ground separation and lightness perception. Perception and Psychophysics 62:1596–618. [SG]Google Scholar
Kim, J. (1998) Mind in a physical world: An essay on the mind-body problem and mental causation. MIT Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1987) Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. In: Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect, ed. Jeannerod, M. North-Holland. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1993) Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In: Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies, ed. Robertson, I. H. & Marshall, J. C. Erlbaum. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kirschfeld, K. (1999) Afterimages: A tool for defining the neural correlate of visual consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8:462–83. [HER]Google Scholar
Knight, G. (2001) Idealism, intentionality, and nonexistent objects. Journal of Philosophical Research 26:4352. [AR]Google Scholar
Koenderinck, J. J. (1990) The brain as a geometry engine. Psychological Research 52:122–27. [VR]Google Scholar
Koffka, K. (1935) Principles of Gestalt psychology. Harcourt Brace/ Kegan, Paul, Trench & Trubner. [CRF, arSL, RL, SM, VR]Google Scholar
Koffka, K. (1969) The task of Gestalt psychology. Princeton University Press. [CRF]Google Scholar
Kohler, I. (1961) Interne und externe Organisation in der Wahrnehmung. Psychologische Beiträge (Festschrift für W. Köhler) 6:426–38. [BD]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1920/1938) Die physische Gestalten in Ruhe und stationären Zustand: Eine naturphilosophische Untersuchung. (Physical Gestalten). In: A source book of Gestalt psychology, ed. Ellis, W. D. (1938). Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Reprinted from Brownschweig: Vieweg & Son.) [SM]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1924) Die physischen Gestalten in Ruhe und im stationären Zustand: Eine naturphilosophische Untersuchung. Verlag der Philosophischen Akadamie. [aSL]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1927/1971) Zum Problem der Regulation (On the problem of regulation). In: The selected papers of Wolfgang Köhler, ed. Henle, M. Liveright. (English edition, 1971). [SM]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1940) Dynamics in psychology. Liveright. [RL, CRF]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1947) Gestalt psychology. Liveright. [SM, CRF]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1960) The mind-body problem. In: Dimension of mind, ed. Hook, S. New York University Press. [RL]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1961) Gestalt psychology today. In: Documents of Gestalt Psychology, ed. Henle, M. University of California Press. [CFR]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1969) The task of Gestalt psychology. Princeton University Press. [aSL, RL]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1971) A task for philosophers. In: The selected papers of Wolfgang Köhler, ed. Henle, Mary Liveright. [arSL]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. & Held, R. (1947) The cortical correlate of pattern vision. Science 110:414–19. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kolb, B. & Whishaw, I. Q. (1996) Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. W. H. Freeman. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kopfermann, H. (1930) Psychologische Untersuchungen über die Wirkung Zweidimensionaler Darstellunger körperliche Gebilde. Psychologische Forschung 13:293364. [JH]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (1975) Information representation in visual images. Cognitive Psychology 7:341–70. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980) Image and mind. Harvard University Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (1994) Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. MIT Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1970) The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago University Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Làdavas, E., Berti, A., Ruozzi, E. & Barboni, F. (1997) Neglect as a deficit determined by an imbalance between spatial representations. Experimental Brain Research 116:493500. [aSL]Google Scholar
Laming, D. (1986) Sensory analysis. Academic Press. [DL]Google Scholar
Laming, D. (1997) The Measurement of sensation. Oxford University Press. [DL]Google Scholar
Laming, D. (2004) Understanding human motivation: What makes people tick? Blackwell. [DL]Google Scholar
Lee, D. N. & Aronson, E. (1974) Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants. Perception and Psychophysics 15:529–32. [PUT]Google Scholar
Lee, D., Georgopoulos, A., Pepping, G.-J. & Lee, T. M. (2002) Information for movement guidance in the nervous system. Paper presented at the Seventh European Workshop on Ecological Psychology, Bendor Island, France, July 46, 2002. [CRF]Google Scholar
Lehar, S. (2003a) Directional harmonic theory: A computational Gestalt model to account for illusory contour and vertex formation. Perception 32:423448. Also available at: http://cns-alumni.bu.edu/~slehar/webstuff/dirhr1/dirhr1.html. [aSL]Google Scholar
Lehar, S. (2003b) The world in your head: A Gestalt view of the mechanism of conscious experience. Erlbaum. [arSL]Google Scholar
Lesher, G. W. (1995) Illusory contours: Toward a neurally based perceptual theory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 2:279321. [aSL]Google Scholar
Levitt, J. B. & Lund, J. S. (1997) Contrast dependence of contextual effects in primate visual cortex. Nature 387:7376. [NPM]Google Scholar
Livingstone, M. & Hubel, D. (1988) Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science 240:740–49. [NPM]Google Scholar
Llinas, R. R., Ribary, U., Joliot, M. & Wang, X.-J. (1994) Content and context in temporal thalamocortical binding. In: Temporal coding in the brain, ed. Buzsaki, G., Llinas, R. R. & Singer, W. Springer-Verlag. [aSL]Google Scholar
Lloyd, D. (2002) Functional MRI and the study of human consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14:818–31. [DLl]Google Scholar
Lloyd, D. (2003) Radiant cool: A novel theory of consciousness. MIT Press. [DLl]Google Scholar
Logvinenko, A., Kane, J. & Ross, D. A. (2002) Is lightness induction a pictorial illusion? Perception 31:7382. [JAS]Google Scholar
Löw, R. (1984) The metaphysical limits of evolutionary epistemology. In: Concepts and approaches in evolutionary epistemology, ed. Wuketits, F. Reidel. [AR]Google Scholar
Luchins, A. S. & Luchins, E. H. (1982) An introduction to the origins of Wertheimer's Gestalt psychology. Gestalt Theory 4:145–71. [RL]Google Scholar
Luchins, A. S. & Luchins, E. H. (1999) Isomorphism in Gestalt theory: Comparison of Wertheimer's and Köhler's concepts. Gestalt Theory 21:208–34. [RL]Google Scholar
Luneburg, R. K. (1950) The metric of binocular visual space. Journal of the Optical Society of America 40:627–42. [aSL]Google Scholar
Lyons, W. (1986) The disappearance of introspection. MIT Press. [DAB]Google Scholar
Mack, A. & Rock, I. (1998) Inattentional blindness. MIT Press. [DLl]Google Scholar
Marković, S. & Gvozdenović, V. (2001) Symmetry, complexity and perceptual economy: Effects of minimum and maximum simplicity conditions. Visual Cognition 8:305–27. [SM]Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982) Vision. W. H. Freeman. [SM]Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Poggio, T. (1976) Cooperative computation of stereo disparity. Science 194:283–87. [aSL]Google Scholar
Marshall, J. C. & Halligan, P. W. (1995) Seeing the forest but only half the trees? Nature 373:521–23. [aSL]Google Scholar
Maund, B. (1975) The representative theory of perception. Canadian Journal of Philosophy 5:4155. [EW]Google Scholar
Maund, B. (1995) Colours: their nature and representation. Cambridge University Press. [EW]Google Scholar
McCready, D. (1985) On size, distance, and visual angle perception. Perception and Psychophysics 37:323–34. [HER]Google Scholar
McFie, J. & Zangwill, O. L. (1960) Visual-constructive disabilities associated with lesions of the left cerebral hemisphere. Brain 83:243–60. [aSL]Google Scholar
McLoughlin, N. & Grossberg, S. (1998) Cortical computation of stereo disparity. Vision Research 38:9199. [SG]Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (1936) Gesetze des Sehens. W. Kramer. [BD]Google Scholar
Michaels, C. & Carello, C. (1981) Direct perception. Prentice-Hall. [RL]Google Scholar
Millar, A. (1991) Reasons and experience. Clarendon Press. [EW]Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [JH]Google Scholar
Milner, A. D. & Goodale, M. A. (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press. [CRF, aSL]Google Scholar
Mitchison, G. (1993) The neural representation of stereoscopic depth contrast. Perception 22:1415–26. [aSL]Google Scholar
Morel, J. M. & Solimini, S. (1995) Variational methods in image segmentation. Birckhaüser. [VR]Google Scholar
Moutoussis, K. & Zeki, S. (2002) The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99:9527–32. [WAM]Google Scholar
Movshon, J. A., Adelson, E. H., Gizzi, M. S. & Newsome, W. T. (1986) The analysis of moving patterns. In: Pattern recognition mechanisms, ed. Chagas, C., Gattass, R. & Cross, C. Springer Verlag. [aSL]Google Scholar
Müller, G. E. (1896) Zur Psychophysik der Gesichtsempfindungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 10. [aSL]Google Scholar
Munk, M. H. J., Roelfsema, P. R., König, P., Engel, A. K. & Singer, W. (1996) Role of reticular activation in the modulation of intracortical synchronization. Science 272:271–74. [WAM]Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1965) Physicalism. Philosophical Review 74:339–56. [KG]Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974) What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 83:435–50. [KG, aSL]Google Scholar
Olson, C. & Gettner, S. (1995) Object-centered direction selectivity in the macaque supplementary eye field. Science 269:985–88. [CRF]Google Scholar
Opie, J. (1999) Gestalt theories of cognitive representation and processing. Psycoloquy 10(021). [aSL]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1992) Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology 46:461–88. [arSL, NPM, PUT]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. & Noë, A. (2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24:883975. [WD, JH]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. & Noë, A. (2002) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24:793810. [JH, EW]Google Scholar
Pace-Schott, E. F. & Hobson, J. A. (2002) The neurobiology of sleep: Genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3:591605. [WAM]Google Scholar
Palmer, S. E. (1992) Modern theories of Gestalt perception. In: Understanding vision, ed. Humphreys, G. W. Blackwell. [aSL]Google Scholar
Palmer, S. E. (1999) Color, consciousness, and the isomorphism constraint. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:121. [aSL]Google Scholar
Parks, R. W., Levine, D. S. & Long, D., ed. (1998) Fundamentals of neural network modeling. MIT Press. [WD]Google Scholar
Penrose, L. & Penrose, R. (1958) Impossible objects: A special type of visual illusion. British Journal of Psychology 49:3133. [JH]Google Scholar
Pessoa, L., Thompson, E. & Noë, A. (1998) Finding out about filling-in: A guide to perceptual completion for visual science and the philosophy of perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:723802. [aSL]Google Scholar
Peterson, M. A. (1994) Shape recognition can and does occur before figure-ground organization. Current Directions in Psychological Science 3:105–11. [JH]Google Scholar
Peterson, M. A. & Gibson, B. S. (1993) Shape recognition contributions to figureground organization in three-dimensional display. Cognitive Psychology 25:383429. [JH]Google Scholar
Petitot, J. (1999) Morphological eidetics. In: Naturalizing phenomenology. Issues in contemporary phenomenology and cognitive science, ed. Petitot, J., Varela, F., Pachoud, B., & Roy, J. M. Stanford University Press. [VR]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1980) Mental imagery and the third dimension. Journal of Experimental Psychology 109:354–71. [aSL]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1988) A computational theory of the mental imagery medium. In: Cognitive and neuropsychological approaches to mental imagery, ed. Denis, M., Engelkamp, J. & Richardson, J. T. E. Martinus Nijhoff. [aSL]Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1905/2001) The value of science: essential writings of Henri Poincaré. Modern Library. [VR]Google Scholar
Previc, F. H. (1998) The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychological Bulletin 124:123–64. [HER]Google Scholar
Pribram, K. H. (1971) Languages of the brain: Experimental paradoxes and principles in neuropsychology. Brandon House. [MV]Google Scholar
Price, H. H. (1932) Perception. Methuen. [aSL]Google Scholar
Pritchard, R. M. (1961) Stabilized images on the retina. Scientific American 204:7278. [BD]Google Scholar
Pritchard, R. M., Heron, W. & Hebb, D. O. (1961) Visual perception approached by the method of stabilized images. Canadian Journal of Psychology 14:6777. [BD]Google Scholar
Raizada, R. D. S. & Grossberg, S. (2001) Context-sensitive binding by the laminar circuits of V1 and V2: A unified model of perceptual grouping, attention, and orientation contrast. Visual Cognition 8:431–66. [SG]Google Scholar
Raizada, R. D. S. & Grossberg, S. (2003) Towards a theory of the laminar architecture of cerebral cortex: Computational clues from the visual system. Cerebral Cortex 13:100–13. [SG]Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (1992) Filling in gaps in perception: Part 1. Current Directions in Psychological Science 1:199205. [aSL]Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. & Anstis, S. M. (1986) The perception of apparent motion. Scientific American 254:8087. [aSL]Google Scholar
Randrup, A. (1997) An alternative to materialism. Cybernetics and Human Knowing 4:1524. [AR]Google Scholar
Randrup, A. (2002) What is real? Conscious experience seen as basic to ontology. An overview [On-line]. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00002660/01/reality.html and http://www.mobilixnet.dk/~mob79301/reality.html. [AR]Google Scholar
Randrup, A. (submitted) An idealist approach to the study of evolution and cognition. Evolution and Cognition. [AR]Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (1995) Consciousness, dreams, and virtual realities. Philosophical Psychology 8:3558. [aSL, AR, MV]Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (1998) Visual perception and subjective visual awareness. Commentary on Pessoa et al. (1998). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:769–70. [aSL, AR]Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (2000) Prospects for a scientific research program on consciousness. In: Neural correlates of consciousness, ed. Metzinger, T. MIT Press. [AR]Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (2001a) Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain? Journal of Consciousness Studies 8:323. [AR]Google Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (2001b) Dreaming and the place of consciousness in nature. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24:10001001. [AR]Google Scholar
Richards, W. (1977) Lessons in constancy from neurophysiology. In: Stability and constancy in visual perception, ed. Epstein, W. Wiley. [HER]Google Scholar
Riehle, A., Grammont, F., Diesmann, M. & Grün, S. (2000) Dynamical changes and temporal precision of synchronized spiking activity in monkey motor cortex during movement preparation. Journal of Physiology (Paris) 94:569–82. [WAM]Google Scholar
Rock, I. & Brosgole, L. (1964) Grouping based on phenomenal proximity. Journal of Experimental Psychology 67:531–38. [aSL]Google Scholar
Rock, I. & Brosgole, L. (1983) Logic of perception. MIT Press. [SM]Google Scholar
Roland, P. E. (2002) Dynamic depolarization fields in the cerebral cortex. Trends in Neurosciences 25:183–90. [WAM]Google Scholar
Rosenberg, G. (2003) A place for consciousness: The theory of natural individuals. Oxford University Press (forthcoming). [aSL]Google Scholar
Rosenthal, V. & Visetti, Y. M. (2003) Köhler. Les Belles Lettres. [VR]Google Scholar
Ross, H. E. & Plug, C. (2002) The mystery of the moon illusion: Exploring size perception. Oxford University Press. [HER]Google Scholar
Ross, W., Grossberg, S. & Mingolla, E. (2000) Visual cortical mechanisms of perceptual grouping: Interacting layers, networks, columns, and maps. Neural Networks 13:571–88. [SG]Google Scholar
Rubin, E. (1921) Visuell wahrgenommene Figuren. Gyldendalska. [DL]Google Scholar
Rubin, N. (2001) Figure and ground in the brain. Nature Neuroscience 4:857–58. [WAM]Google Scholar
Ruse, M. (1986) Taking Darwin seriously. Basil Blackwell. [AR]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1921) The analysis of mind. George Allen & Unwin. [aSL]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1927) Philosophy. W. W. Norton. [arSL]Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949) The concept of mind. Hutchinson's University Library. [DL]Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949/1966) The concept of mind. Penguin Books. [EW]Google Scholar
Sacks, O. (1985) The man who mistook his wife for a hat. Harper & Row. [aSL]Google Scholar
Schirillo, J., Reeves, A. & Arend, L. (1990) Perceived lightness, but not brightness, of achromatic surfaces depends on perceived depth information. Perception and Psychophysics 48:8290. [JAS]Google Scholar
Schwartz, E. L. (1980) Computational anatomy and functional architecture of striate cortex: A spatial mapping approach to perceptual coding. Vision Research 20:645–69. [HER]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1980) Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:417–24. [aSL]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1992) The rediscovery of mind. The MIT Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1997) The mystery of consciousness. New York Review. [aSL]Google Scholar
Sellars, R. W. (1916) Critical realism: A study of the nature and conditions of knowledge. Rand McNally. [aSL, EW]Google Scholar
Sellars, R. W. (1922) Evolutionary naturalism. Open Court. [EW]Google Scholar
Sellars, R. W. (1970) The principles, perspectives and problems of philosophy. Pageant Press International. [EW]Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E. (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Systems Technical Journal 27:379423. [aSL]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. E. & Bransford, J. (1977) Introduction: Psychological approaches to the problem of knowledge. In: Perceiving, acting and knowing, ed. Shaw, R. & Bransford, J. Erlbaum. [SM]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. E. & Turvey, M. T. (1981) Coalitions as models for ecosystems: A realist perspective on perceptual organization. In: Perceptual organization, ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J. R. Erlbaum. [SM]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1981) Psychophysical complementarity. In: Perceptual organization, ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomeranz, J. Erlbaum. [aSL]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1987) Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237:1317–23. [WD]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1994) Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 1:228 (1994). Reprinted in: Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(4):581–601. Special Issue on the work of Roger Shepard (2001). [WD]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. & Chipman, S. (1970) Second-order isomorphism of internal representations: Shapes of states. Cognitive Psychology 1:117. [aSL]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. & Metzler, J. (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701703. [aSL]Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., ed. (2000) Change blindness and visual memory: A special issue of the Journal of Visual Cognition. Psychology Press. [DLl]Google Scholar
Singer, W. (1999) Neuronal synchrony: A versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 24:4965. [aSL, SM]Google Scholar
Singer, W. & Gray, C. (1995) Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18:555–86. [aSL]Google Scholar
Singh, M. & Hoffman, D. D. (1998) Active vision and the basketball problem. Commentary on Pessoa et al. (1998). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:772–73. [aSL]Google Scholar
Smythies, J. R. (1989) The mind-brain problem. In: The case for dualism, ed. Smythies, J. R. & Beloff, J. University of Virginia Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Solomon, S. G., White, A. J. & Martin, P. R. (2002) Extraclassical receptive field properties of parvocellular, magnocellular, and koniocellular cells in the primate lateral geniculate nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience 22:338–49. [NPM]Google Scholar
Solomon, S. G., White, A. J. & Martin, P. R. (1994) The walls of Plato's Cave: The science and philosophy of brain, consciousness, and perception. Avebury. [aSL]Google Scholar
Stettler, D. D., Das, A., Bennett, J. & Gilbert, C. D. (2002) Lateral connectivity and contextual interactions in macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron 36:739–50. [NPM]Google Scholar
Stuart, G. W., Bossomaier, T. R. J. & Johnson, S. (1993) Preattentive processing of object size: Implications for theories of size perception. Perception 22:1175–93. [HER]Google Scholar
Swaminathan, G. & Grossberg, S. (2001) Laminar cortical circuits for the perception of slanted and curved 3D surfaces. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts Vol. 619, No. 49. [SG]Google Scholar
Takeichi, H., Watanabe, T. & Shimojo, S. (1992) Illusory occluding contours and surface formation by depth propagation. Perception 21:177–84. [aSL]Google Scholar
Tausch, R. (1954) Optische Täyschungen als artifizielle Effekte der Gestaltungsprozesse von Grössen und Formenkonstanz in der natürlichen Raumwahrnehmung. Psychologische Forschung 24:299348. [aSL]Google Scholar
Taylor, J. G. (1999) The race for consciousness. MIT Press. [WD]Google Scholar
Thau, M. (2002) Consciousness and cognition. MIT Press. [EW]Google Scholar
Thiele, A. & Stoner, G. (2003) Neuronal synchrony does not correlate with motion coherence in cortical area MT. Nature 421:266–70. [WAM]Google Scholar
Thouless, R. H. (1931a) Phenomenal regression to the “real” object. I. British Journal of Psychology 21:339–59. [DL]Google Scholar
Thouless, R. H. (1931b) Phenomenal regression to the “real” object. II. British Journal of Psychology 22:130. [DL]Google Scholar
Trehub, A. (1991) The cognitive brain. MIT Press. [HER]Google Scholar
Tse, P. U. (1999a) Illusory volumes from conformation. Perception 27:(8):977–94. [aSL]Google Scholar
Tse, P. U. (1999b) Volume completion. Cognitive Psychology 39:3768. [aSL, PUT]Google Scholar
Tse, P. U. & Logothetis, N. K. (2002) The duration of 3-D form analysis in transformational apparent motion. Perception and Psychophysics 64:244–65. [PUT]Google Scholar
Vallar, G. (1998) Spatial hemineglect in humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(3):8796. [aSL]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1990) Consciousness, brain, and the physical world. Philosophical Psychology 3:7799. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000238/. [aSL,MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1991a) Consciousness from a first-person perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:702–26. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000594/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1991b) Is human information processing conscious? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:651–69. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000593/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1993) A reflexive science of consciousness. In: Experimental and theoretical studies of consciousness. CIBA Foundation Symposium 174. Wiley. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000243/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1996) Consciousness and the “causal paradox.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19:537–42. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000596/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (2000) Understanding consciousness. Routledge/Psychology Press. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (2001) A natural account of phenomenal consciousness. Communication and Cognition 34:3959. Available from http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00001813/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (2002a) How could conscious experiences affect brains? Journal of Consciousness Studies 9:329. http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00002750/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (2002b) Making sense of the causal interactions between consciousness and brain (a reply to commentaries). Journal of Consciousness Studies 9:6995. http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00002751/. [MV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (2003) Is the world in the brain, or the brain in the world? (Unabridged version of this Behavioral and Brain Sciences paper) http://cogprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00002798/commentary). [MV]Google Scholar
von Ehrenfels, C. (1890) Über Gestaltqualitäten. Vierteljahresschrift für wissenschaftliche Philosophie 14:249–92. [BD]Google Scholar
Ware, C. & Kennedy, J. M. (1978) Perception of subjective lines, surfaces and volumes in 3-dimensional constructions. Leonardo 11:111–14. [aSL]Google Scholar
Westheimer, G. & Levi, D. M. (1987) Depth attraction and repulsion of disparate foveal stimuli. Vision Research 27:1361–68. [aSL]Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953) Philosophical investigations. Blackwell. [DAB]Google Scholar
Wright, E. L. (1990) Two more proofs of present qualia. Theoria 60:322. [EW]Google Scholar
Wright, E. L. (1992) The entity fallacy in epistemology. Philosophy 67:3350. [EW]Google Scholar
Wright, E. L. (1996) What it isn't like. American Philosophical Quarterly 33:2342. [EW]Google Scholar
Wuketits, F. M. (1984) Concepts and approaches in evolutionary epistemology. Reidel. [AR]Google Scholar
Yarbus, A. L. (1967) Eye movements and vision. Plenum Press. [aSL]Google Scholar
Zeki, S. & Bartels, A. (1998) The asynchrony of consciousness. Proceedings of the Royal Society (London) Series B 265:1583–85. [WAM]Google Scholar
Zucker, S. W., David, C., Dobbins, A. & Iverson, L. (1988) The organization of curve detection: Coarse tangent fields and fine spline coverings. In: Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Computer Vision, IEEE Computer Society, Tampa FL, pp. 568–77. http://www.cs.yale.edu/homes/vision/zucker/publications_full.html [aSL]Google Scholar