Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T21:06:26.922Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why limit the availability of a prime-word in the study of automatic contextual facilitation?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Juan Segui
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie Expérimentale, Université René Descartes, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Paris 75006, France
Cécile Beauvillain
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie Expérimentale, Université René Descartes, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Paris 75006, France

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Continuing Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balota, D. A. (1983) Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Beahvior 22: 88104. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A. (1986) Unconscious semantic processing: The pendulum keeps on swinging. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 2324. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A. & Chumley, J. I. (1984) Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10: 340–57. [DH]Google ScholarPubMed
Breitmeyer, B. G. (1984) Visual masking: An integrative approach. Oxford University Press. [DH]Google Scholar
Brown, J. W. (1977) Mind, brain and consciousness. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Brown, J. W. (1982) Hierarchy and evolution in neurolinguistics. In: Neural models of language processes, ed. Arbib, M., Caplan, D. & Marshall, J. C.. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Brown, J. W. (1983) Rethinking the right hemisphere. In: Cognitive processing in the right hemisphere, ed. Perecman, E.. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Brown, W. P. (1961) Conceptions of perceptual defence. British Journal of Psychology. Monograph Supplement No. 35. [JAG]Google Scholar
Cheesman, J. & Merikle, P. M. (1985) Word recognition and consciousness. In: Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, vol. 5, ed. Besner, D., Waller, T. G. & MacKinnon, G. E.. Academic Press. [DH]Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Segui, J. (1986) The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language 25: 385400. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B. (1983) The range of automatic spreading activation in word priming. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22: 417–36. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B., Thomassen, A. J. W. M. & Hudson, P. T. W. (1982) Associative facilitation of word recognition as measured from a neutral prime. Memory and Cognition 10: 358–70. [JS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M. (1980a) Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemideeortication. Brain and Language 10: 287317. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M. (1980b) Language acquisition in a single hemisphere: Semantic organization. In: Biological studies of mental processes, ed. Caplan, D.. MIT Press. [DH]Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Lovett, M. & Wiegel-Crump, C. A. (1981) Written language acquisition after left or right hemideeortication in infancy. Brain and Language 12: 5491. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, N. F. (1971) Subliminal perception. McGraw-Hill. [DH]Google Scholar
Dixon, N. F. (1981) Preconsdous processing. Wiley. [JAG, DH, GK]Google Scholar
Dixon, N. F. (1986) On private events and brain events. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 2930. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erdelyi, M. H. (1986) Experimental indeterminacies in the dissociation paradigm of subliminal perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 3031. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksen, C. W. (1960) Discrimination and learning without awareness: A methodological survey and evaluation. Psychological Review 67: 279300. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evett, L. J., Humphreys, G. W. & Quinlan, P. T. (1986) Identification, masking, and priming: Clarifying the issues. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 3132. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT Press. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1985) Precis of The modularity of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 142. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1979) Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In: Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett, ed. Cooper, W. E. & Walker, E. C. T.. Erlbaum. [JS]Google Scholar
Fowler, C. A., Wolford, G., Slade, R. & Tassinary, L. (1981) Lexical access with and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110: 341–62. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1983) Right hemisphere language following brain bisection. American Psychologist 38: 525–37. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. & Odom, J. V. (1984) Comparison of monoptic and dichoptic masking by light. Perception and Psychophysics 35: 265–68. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groeger, J. A. (1984) Evidence of unconscious semantic processing from a forced error situation. British Journal of Psychology 75: 305–14. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groeger, J. A. (1986) Predominant and non-predominant analysis: Effects of level of presentation. British Journal of Psychology 77: 109–16. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, S. H. A. (1976) Responses to homophones as a function of cue words on the unattended channel. British Journal of Psychology 67: 559–67. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, S. H. A. & Dixon, N. F. (1974) Laterality differences in the effects of incidental stimuli upon evoked imagery. British Journal of Psychology 65: 529–36. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holender, D. (1986) Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J. & Taylor, D. E. (1982) Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition 10: 576–90. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinsbourne, M. (1985) Parallel processing explains modular informational encapsulation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 23. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1966) The alternation process in binocular rivalry. British Journal of Psychology 57: 225–38. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macmillan, N. A. (1986) The psychophysics of subliminal perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 3839. [DH]Google Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology 15: 197237. [JAG, DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcel, A. J. (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relation between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238300. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1986) Consciousness and processing: Choosing and testing a null hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 4041. [DH]Google Scholar
Merikle, P. M. (1982) Unconscious perception revised. Perception and Psychophysics 31: 298301. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikle, P. M. & Cheesman, J. (1986) Consciousness is a “subjective” state. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 42. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moray, N. (1959) Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psycholog 11: 5660. [JMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moseovitch, M. (1983) The linguistic and emotional functions of the normal right hemisphere. In: Cognitive processing in the right hemisphere, ed. Perecman, E.. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Mykel, N. & Daves, W. F. (1979) Emergence of unreported stimuli into imagery as a function of laterality of presentation: A replication and extension of research by Henley and Dixon. British Journal of Psychology 70: 253–58. [JAG, DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philpott, A. & Wilding, J. (1979) Semantic interference from subliminal stimuli in a dichoptic viewing situation. British Journal of Psychology 70: 559–63. [DH, JMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. (1975a) Attention and cognitive control. In: Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium, ed. Solso, R. L.. Erlbaum. [JS]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. (1975b) Facilitation and inhibition in the processing of signals. In: Attention and Performance V, ed. Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Dornic, S.. Academic Press. [JS]Google Scholar
Reicher, G. M. (1969) Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology 81: 275–80. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G. J. W., Spence, D. P. & Klein, G. S. (1959) Subliminal effects of verbal stimuli. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59: 167–76. [DH]Google ScholarPubMed
Somekh, D. E. & Wilding, J. M. (1973) Perception without awareness in a dichoptic viewing situation. British Journal of Psychology 64: 339–49. [JAG, DH, JMW]Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & Purcell, D. G. (1986) Priming without awareness: What was all the fuss about? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 4748. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. & Geffen, G. (1967) Selective attention: Perception or response? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 19: 117. [JMW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turvey, M. T. (1973) On peripheral and central processes in vision: Interference from an information-processing analysis of masking with patterned stimuli. Psychological Review 80: 152. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, G. & Thwaites, S. (1982) Automatic phonological coding of unattended printed words. Memory and Cognition 10: 434–42. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilding, J. M. & Farrell, J. M. (1970) Selective attention: Superior detection of word targets compared with sound targets in a prose passage while shadowing another passage. Psychonomic Science 19: 123–24. [DH, JMW]Google Scholar
Walker, P. (1978) Binocular rivalry: Central or peripheral selective processes? Psychological Bulletin 85: 376–89. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. (1986) Stereopsis and binocular rivalry. Psychological Review 93: 269–82. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimba, L. D. & Blake, R. (1983) Binocular rivalry and semantic processing: Out of sight, out of mind. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9: 807–15. [DH, JMW]Google Scholar