Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Shettleworth, Sara J.
2012.
Modularity, comparative cognition and human uniqueness.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
Vol. 367,
Issue. 1603,
p.
2794.
McCaffrey, Joseph
and
Machery, Edouard
2012.
Philosophical issues about concepts.
WIREs Cognitive Science,
Vol. 3,
Issue. 2,
p.
265.
Abrahamson, Dor
2012.
Seeing chance: perceptual reasoning as an epistemic resource for grounding compound event spaces.
ZDM,
Vol. 44,
Issue. 7,
p.
869.
Weng, Juyang
and
Luciw, Matthew
2012.
Brain-Like Emergent Spatial Processing.
IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 2,
p.
161.
Machamer, Peter
and
Osbeck, Lisa
2012.
Perception, Realism, and the Problem of Reference.
p.
142.
Heintz, Christophe
2012.
From core cognition to intuitive theories: A psychologist’s account of conceptual change.
Metascience,
Vol. 21,
Issue. 2,
p.
439.
Umbres, Radu
2013.
Chasse au dahu et vigilance épistémique.
Terrain,
Vol. 61,
Issue. ,
p.
84.
Abrahamson, Dor
2013.
Toward a taxonomy of design genres.
p.
218.
Syrett, Kristen
and
Musolino, Julien
2013.
Collectivity, Distributivity, and the Interpretation of Plural Numerical Expressions in Child and Adult Language.
Language Acquisition,
Vol. 20,
Issue. 4,
p.
259.
Sun, Ron
2013.
Autonomous generation of symbolic representations through subsymbolic activities.
Philosophical Psychology,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 6,
p.
888.
Russell, Yvan I.
and
Gobet, Fernand
2013.
What is Counterintuitive? Religious Cognition and Natural Expectation.
Review of Philosophy and Psychology,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 4,
p.
715.
Ziv, Ido
and
Leiser, David
2013.
The need for central resources in answering questions in different domains: Folk psychology, biology, and economics.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
Vol. 25,
Issue. 7,
p.
816.
Reigosa-Crespo, Vivian
González-Alemañy, Eduardo
León, Teresa
Torres, Rosario
Mosquera, Raysil
Valdés-Sosa, Mitchell
and
Allen, Philip
2013.
Numerical Capacities as Domain-Specific Predictors beyond Early Mathematics Learning: A Longitudinal Study.
PLoS ONE,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 11,
p.
e79711.
Hoffmann, Danielle
Hornung, Caroline
Martin, Romain
and
Schiltz, Christine
2013.
Developing number–space associations: SNARC effects using a color discrimination task in 5-year-olds.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Vol. 116,
Issue. 4,
p.
775.
Котов, Алексей
and
Дагаев, Николай
2013.
Роль предыдущих знаний в порождении эффекта понятийной гибкости.
Психологические исследования,
Vol. 6,
Issue. 29,
Rusanen, Anna-Mari
2014.
Towards to An Explanation for Conceptual Change: A Mechanistic Alternative.
Science & Education,
Vol. 23,
Issue. 7,
p.
1413.
Abrahamson, Dor
2014.
Building educational activities for understanding: An elaboration on the embodied-design framework and its epistemic grounds.
International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction,
Vol. 2,
Issue. 1,
p.
1.
Hunt, Kathryn
and
Chittka, Lars
2014.
False memory susceptibility is correlated with categorisation ability in humans.
F1000Research,
Vol. 3,
Issue. ,
p.
154.
Hunt, Kathryn
and
Chittka, Lars
2014.
False memory susceptibility is correlated with categorisation ability in humans.
F1000Research,
Vol. 3,
Issue. ,
p.
154.
Brulé, Laurianne
Labrell, Florence
Megalakaki, Olga
Fouquet, Nathalie
and
Caillies, Stéphanie
2014.
Children's justifications of plants as living things between 5 and 7 years of age.
European Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Vol. 11,
Issue. 5,
p.
532.
Target article
What is the significance of The Origin of Concepts for philosophers' and psychologists' theories of concepts?
Related commentaries (1)
Précis of The Origin of Concepts