No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Language: levels of characterisation
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980
References
Adinolfi, M. (1978) The immune system and the brain. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 20:509–16. [JCM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andor, J. (1978) Case grammar, deep semantic relations and cognition. In: Dressier, W. U. and Meid, W., (eds.) Proceedings of the Twelfth Interna tional Congress of Linguists. pp. 163–66. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker Beiträge zur Sprachwissenschaft, [JA]Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N., and Pisoni, D. B. (1980) Some developmental processes in speech perception. In: Yeni-Komshian, G. H.; Kavanagh, J. F.; and Ferguson, C. A. (eds.) Child phonology: perception and production. New York: Academic Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Bard, J. B. L. (1977) A unity underlying the different zebra striping patterns. Journal of Zoology, London 183:527–39. [JCM]Google Scholar
Beilin, H. (1975) Studies in the cognitive basis of language development. New York: Academic Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1967) The acquisition of the system of negation in children's speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Harvard University. [RFC]Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1971) Simplification in children's language. In: Huxley, R. and Ingram, E. (eds.) Language acquisition: models and methods, pp. 95–119. London and New York: Academic Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Bever, T. G. (1970) The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In: Hayes, J. B. (ed.) Cognition and the development of language, pp. 279–362. New York: John Wiley & Sons [RFC]Google Scholar
Bever, T. G.; Lackner, J. R.; and Kirk, R. (1969) The underlying structures of sentences are the primary units of immediate speech processing. Perception and Psychophysics 5:225–34. [GH]Google Scholar
Blakemore, C. (1973) Developmental factors in the formation of feature extracting neurons. In: Schmitt, F. O. and Worden, F. C. (eds.) The neurosciences: third study program. Cambridge: MIT Press [NC]Google Scholar
Blank, M.; Gessner, M.; and Esposito, A. (1978) Language without communication: a case study (Mimeo). Dept. of Psychiatry, Rutgers Medical Sch. Piscataway, N.J. [NC]Google Scholar
Bower, G. H.; Black, J. B.; and Turner, T. J. (1979) Scripts in text comprehension and memory. Cognitive Psychology 11:177–220. [RCS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, D.; Garrett, M. F.; and Zurif, E. (1979) Syntactic deficits in Broca's Aphasia. In: Caplan, D. (ed.) Biological studies of mental processes. Cambridge: MIT Press. [JM]Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. (1978) A realistic transformational grammar. In: Halle, M.; Bresnan, J. and Miller, I. (eds.) Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge: MIT Press. [JM]Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S.; Goodnow, J. J.; and Austin, G. A. (1956) A study of thinking. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [RFC]Google Scholar
Bryant, P. (1974) Perception and understanding in young children: an experimental approach. New York: Basic Books. [NC]Google Scholar
Catlin, J. (1978) Discussion of the chapters by Stolzenberg and Chomsky. In: Miller, C. A. and Lenneberg, E. (eds.) Psychology and biology of language and thought, pp. 271–80. New York: Academic Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1962) Explanatory models in linguistics. In: Nagel, E.; Suppes, P.; and Tarski, A., (eds.) Logic, methodology, and philosophy of science. Stanford: Stanford Univ. Press. [RJM]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press. [NC, JDM. RJM]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1967) The formal nature of language. In: Lenneberg, E. H. (ed.) Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley. [NC, RFC]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1969) Some empirical assumptions in modern philosophy of language. Morgenbesser, S.; Suppes, P.; and White, M. (eds.) Philosophy, Science and Method (Essays in honor of Ernest Nagel) New York: St. Martin's Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1972a) Studies on semantics in generative grammar. The Hague. Mouton. [NC]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1975b) Knowledge of language. In: Cunderson, K. (ed.) Language, mind and knowledge. Minneapolis. Univ. of Minnesota Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1976) On the nature of language. In: Hamad, S. R.; Steklis, H. D.; and Lancaster, J. (eds.) Origins and evolution of language and speech, p. 280. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. [NC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1977) Essays on form and Interpretation. New York: North Holland Elsevier. [NC]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1980) Rules and representations. New York: Columbia Univ. Press. [NC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsk, N. (Forthcoming) Human language and other semiotic systems. Semiotica. [NC]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Halle, , Morris, (1968) The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row. [JDM]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Lasnik, H. (1977) Filters and control. Linguistic Inquiry 7:425–504. [JCM]Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1971) On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 10:266–75. [RFC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973a) How children describe time and order. In: Ferguson, C. A. and Slobin, D. I. (eds.) Studies of child language development, pp. 585–606. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. [RFC]Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973b) Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meaning. Cognition 2:161–82. [RFC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M.; Patterson, K.; and Marshall, J. C., (eds.) (1980) Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. [JCM]Google Scholar
Cooper, D. (1975) Knowledge of language. New York: Prism Press, London and Humanities Press. [SPS]Google Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1968) The development of temporal reference during the acquisition of language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University. [RFC]Google Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1974) The development of language and cognition: the cognition hypothesis. In: Foss, B. (ed.) New perspectives in child development, pp. 184–252. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books. [RFC]Google Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1976a) The cognitive hypothesis of language acquisition and its implications for child language deficiency. In: Morehead, D. M., and Morehead, A. E. (eds) Normal and deficient child language, pp. 283–333. Baltimore, Maryland: University Park Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1976b) Developmental strategies for language. In: Hamilton, V., and Vernon, M. D. (eds.) The development of cognitive processes, pp. 305–58. London and New York: Academic Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (in press) Reconceptualizing language acquisition and cognitive development. In: Schiefelbusch, R. L., and Bricker, D. (eds.) Early language intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Curtiss, S. (1977) Genie: a psycholinguistic study of a modern-day “wild child”. New York: Academic Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Danto, A. G., and Morgenbesser, S. (1957) Character and free will. Journal of Philosophy 54:502. [NC]Google Scholar
Davis, L., and Gardner, H. (1976) Strategies of mastering a visual communication system in aphasia. In: Hamad, S. R.; Steklis, H. D.; and Lancaster, J. (eds.) Origins and evolution of language and speech. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: 280. [NC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denenberg, V. H.; Garbanati, J.; Sherman, G.; Yutzey, D. A.; and Kaplan, R. (1978) Infantile stimulation induces brain lateralization in rats. Science 201:1150–51. [NC]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1975) Why the law of effect will not go away. Journal of the Theory of Social Behavior 5:169–87. [NC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, M. (1978) Language acquisition in a single hemisphere: semantic organization. In: Caplan, D. (ed.) Biological studies of mental processes. Cambridge: MIT Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1980) Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemidecortication. Brain and Language 10. [JCM]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. and Whitaker, H. (1976) Language, acquisition following hemidecorticalion: linguistic superiority of the left over right hemisphere. Brain and Language 3:404–33. [NC]Google Scholar
Dewart, M. H. (1975) A psychological investigation of sentence comprehension by children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University College, London. [RFC]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. and Balfour, G. (1968) Less is more: a study of language comprehension in children. British Journal of Psychology 59:461–72. [RFC]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. and McCarrigle, J. (1974) Some clues to the nature of semantic development. Journal of Child Language 1:185–94. [RFC]Google Scholar
Dresher, B. E., and Hornstein, N. (1976) On some supposed contributions of artificial intelligence to the scientific study of language. Cognition 4:321–398. [NC]Google Scholar
Dresher, B. E., and Hornstein, N. (1977) Reply to Schank and Wilensky. Cognition 5:147–49. [NC]Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. (1971) Les relations temporelles dans le langage de l'enfant. Genève: Librairie Droz. [RFC]Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E., and Sinclair, H. (1971) Temporal relations in language. International Journal of Psychology 6: 39–47. [RFC]Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1975) An alternative to checklist theories of meaning. In: Cogen, C. et al. (eds.) Proceedings of the First Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, pp. 123–31. [JA]Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1977) The case for case reopened. In: Cole, P. and Sadock, J. M. (eds.) Syntax and Semantics Vol. 8., Grammatical Relations, pp. 59–81. New York: Academic Press. [JA]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D. (1978) Parsing strategies and constraints on transformations. Linguistic Inquiry 9:427–73. [JCM]Google Scholar
Frankfurt, , Harry, , (1977) Descartes on the creation of the eternal truths. The Philosophical Review, LXXXVI:36–57. [DMR]Google Scholar
Galaburda, A. M.; Sanides, F.; and Geschwind, N. (1978) Human Brain: Cytoarchitectonic left-right asymmetries in the temporal speech region. Archives of Neurology 35:812–17. [JCM]Google Scholar
Gardner, H.; Zurif, E.; Berry, T.; and Baker, E. (1976) Visual communication in aphasia. Neuropsychologia 14:275–92. [NC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghiselin, M. T. (1974) A radical solution to the species problem. Systematic Zoology 23:536–44. [MTG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1980 In press) Natural kinds and literary accomplishments. Michigan Quarterly Review. [MTG]Google Scholar
Glass, A. V.; Gazzaniga, M. S.; and Premack, D. (1973) Artificial language training in global aphasics. Neuropsychologia 11:95–103. [NC, RFC]Google Scholar
Goosens, W. K. (1978) Reduction by molecular genetics. Philosophy of Science 45:73–95. [JCM]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976a) The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In.: Gottlieb, G. (ed.) Development of neural and behavioral specificity. New York: Academic Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976b) Conceptions of prenatal development: behavioral embryology. Psychological Review 83:215–34. [GG]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (in press) Development of species identification in ducklings: VI. Specific embryonic experience required to maintain species-typical perception in Peking ducklings. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. [GG]Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C.; Gordon, S. E.; and Sawyer, J. D. (1979) Recognition memory for typical and atypical actions in scripted activities: tests of a script pointer and tag hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18:319–32. [RCS]Google Scholar
Graves, C.; Katz, J. J.; Nishiyama, Y.; Soames, S.; Sleeker, B.; and Tovey, P. (1973) Tacit knowledge. Journal of Philosophy 70. [SPS]Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1957) Logic and Conversation. Unpublished William James lectures, Harvard Univ. [JRS]Google Scholar
Harnad, S. R.; Steklis, H. D.; and Lancaster, J. (eds.) (1976) Origins and evolution of language and speech. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: 280. [NC]Google Scholar
Hill, J. H., and Most, R. B. (1978) Review of Hamnard, Steklis, and Lancaster (1976) Language 54:651–2. [NC]Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. (1978) Vision and the brain. Bulletin of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences. 31:28. [NC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, J. (1972) Language and communication: acquisition of a non-vocal “language” by previously languageless children. Unpublished Bachelor of Technology thesis, Brunel University. [RFC]Google Scholar
Hughes, J. (1974/1975) Acquisition of a non-vocal “language” by aphasic children. Cognition 3:41–55. [RFC]Google Scholar
Hull, D. L. (1975) Central subjects and historical narratives. History and Theory 14:253–74. [MTG]Google Scholar
Hull, D. L. (1976) Are species really individuals? Systematic Zoology 25:174–91. [MTG]Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., and Strauss, S. (1968) Comprehension and a statement's relation to the situation it describes. Journal of Verbal Learning And Verbal Behaviour 7:300–4. [RFC]Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J.; Eisenberg, K.; and Strauss, S. (1968) Comprehension: relation between perceived actor and logical subject. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 7:527–30. [RFC]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1977) Review-article: Bernstein, The unanswered questions. Language 53:883–894. [NC]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1979) How to keep ninety from rising. Linguistic Inquiry. 10:172–177. [NC]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. and Lerdahl, F. (forthcoming) A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge: MIT Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Jacob, F. (1978) Darwinism reconsidered. Le Monde Sept. 6–8, 1977; translated in Atlas. [NC]Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1979) On the nature of difficulties in spatio-perceptual tasks. British Journal of Psychology 70:351–63. [AM]Google Scholar
Jaisson, P. (1975) L'impregnation dans l'ontogenese des comportements de soins aux cocons chez la jeune formi rousse (Formica polyctena Forst.) Behaviour 52:1–37. [GG]Google Scholar
Jerne, N. K. (1967) Antibodies and learning: selection versus instruction. In: Quarton, G. C.; Melnechuk, T.; and Schmitt, F. O. (eds.) The neurosciences: a study program. New York: Rockefeller Univ. Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1977) The child's construction of a syslem of plurifunctional markers. In: Bullowa, M. (Chair) Language development. Symposium presented at the biennial conference of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development, Pavia, Italy. [RFC]Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1978) The interplay between syntax, semantics, and phonology in language acquisition processes. In: Campbell, B. N., and Smith, P. T. (eds.) Recent advances in the psychology of language: language development and mother-child interaction, pp. 1–23. New York: Plenum Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979) A functional approach to child language: a study of determiners and reference. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Kasher, A. (1977) Foundations of philosophical pragmatics. In: Butts, R. E. and Hintikka, J. (eds.) Basic problems in methodology and linguistics: logic, methodology and philosophy of science, V. Dordrecht: Reidel. [NC]Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1979) Semantic and conceptual development. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press. [NC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, , Anthony, , translator and editor. (1970) Descartes: philosophical letters. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [DMR]Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. and van Dijk, T. A. (1978) Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological review 85:363–94. [JA]Google Scholar
Koehler, O. (1956) The ability of birds to “count.” In: Newman, J. R., (ed.) The world of mathematics. New York: Simon & Schuster. [NC]Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1977) Linguistic gestalts. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. 1050 E. 59 St., Chicago. [GL]Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. [GL]Google Scholar
Lehnert, W. G. (1977) The process of question answering. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [RCS]Google Scholar
Lesky, E. (1970) Structure and function in Gall. Bulletin of the History of Medicine 44:297–314. [JCM]Google Scholar
Lewes, G. H. (1871) The history of philosophy from Thales to Comte. London: Longmans. [JCM]Google Scholar
Lightfoot, D. W. (1979) Principles of diachronic syntax. New York and Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1965) The evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. [RVG]Google Scholar
Lycan, W. (in preparation) Functionalism and psychological laws. [SPS]Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1972) Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. Psychological Review 79:1–13. [RFC]Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. (1973) The effects of stress on the understanding of pronominal co-reference in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2:1–8. [RFC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, M. (1978) A theory of syntactic recognition for natural language. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. [PTWH]Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1976) Early processing of visual information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 275:483–524. [JCM]Google Scholar
Marr, D. and Nishihara, H. K. (1978) Visual information processing: artificial intelligence and the sensorium of sight. Technology Review 81:2–23. [NC]Google Scholar
Matthews, R. (1979) The plausibility of rationalism. To be presented to the annual meeting of the Society for Philosophy and Psychology, 03, 1980. [RJM]Google Scholar
Mehler, J. and Bertendini, J. (1979) Infants' perception of speech and other acoustic stimuli. In: Morton, J. and Marshall, J. (eds.) Psycholinguistic Seties II. Cambridge: MIT Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1977) Frame-system theory. In: Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Wason, P. C. (eds.) Thinking, pp. 355–76. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. [JA]Google Scholar
Moravcsik, J. (1975) Aitia as generative factor in Aristotle's philosophy. Dialogue. [NC]Google Scholar
Moravcsik, J. (1977) “How do words get their meaning?” Yehoshua Bar-Hillel lecture, Jerusalem, forthcoming. [NC]Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1969) Linguistics and epistemology. In: Hook, S. (ed.) Language and Philosophy. New York: New York Univ. Press. [SPS]Google Scholar
Nelson, K., and Gruendel, J. (1978) From person episode to social script: two dimensions in the development of event knowledge. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco. [RCS]Google Scholar
Newport, E.; Gleitman, H.; and Gleitman, L. (1977) Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In: Snow, C. E. and Ferguson, C. A. (eds.) Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Ojemann, G., and Mateer, C. (1979) Human language cortex: localization of memory, syntax, and sequential motor-phoneme identification systems. Science 205:1401–3. [JCM]Google Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, C. (1978a) Chimpanzee problem-solving: a test for comprehension. Science 202:532–35. [NC]Google Scholar
Premacic, D. (1978b) Does the Chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:515–26. [NC]Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1975) Mind and verbal dispositions. In: Guttenplan, S. (ed.) Mind and language. London: Oxford. [NC]Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1979) The role of symmetry in Ghiselin's “radical solution to the species problem.” Systematic Zoology 28:71–78. [MTG]Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1978) Violations of the Wh island constraint in Italian and the subjacency condition. In: Dubisson, C.; Lightfoot, D.; and Morin, Y. C. (eds.) Montreal Working Papers in Linguistics 2:155–90. [NC]Google Scholar
Rollin, B. (1971) Hume's blue patch and the mind's creativity. Journal of the History of Ideas XXXII:119ff. [BER]Google Scholar
Rollin, B. (1976) Natural and conventional meaning: an examination of the distinction. The Hague: Mouton. [BER]Google Scholar
Rollin, B. (1978) Thomas Reid and the semiotics of perception. The Monist 61:257ff. [BER]Google Scholar
Sampson, G. R. (1978) Linguistic universals as evidence for empiricism. Journal of Linguistics 14:183–206. [GS]Google Scholar
Sampson, G. R. (1979) A non-nativist account of language universals. Linguistics and Philosophy 3:99–104. [GS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E. (1933) The psychological reality of the phonene. Reprinted in English translation in: Mandelbaum, D. C. (ed.) Selected writings of Edward Sapir. Berkeley and Los Angeles: Univ. of California Press, 1949. [NC]Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, K. S.; Rumbaugh, D. M.; and Boysen, S. (1978) Symbolic communication between two chimpanzees. Science 201:641–42. [NC]Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1977) Production and comprehension of utterances. Hills- dale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum [JA]Google Scholar
Schlesinger, I. M. (1979) Cognitive structures and semantic deep structures. Journal of Linguistics 15:307–24. [JA]Google Scholar
Schopenhauer, A. (1974) The fourfold root of the principle of sufficient reason. La Salle, Illinois: Open Court. [NC]Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. (1969) On knowing a grammar. In: Hook, S. (ed.) Language and philosophy: New York: New York Univ. Press. [SPS]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1969) Speech acts. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge Univ. Press. [JRS]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1976) The rules of the language game. Review article on Reflections on Language. Times Literary Supplement. 09 10. [NC, JRS]Google Scholar
Selfridge, M. (1979) A process model of language acquisition. Ph.D. Thesis, Department of Computer Science, Yale University, New Haven, CT. [RCS]Google Scholar
Serpell, R. (1979) How specific are perceptual skills? British Journal of Psychology 70:365–80. [AM]Google Scholar
Sheldon, A. (1974) The role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clauses in English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 13:272–81. [RFC]Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1962) Thearchitectureof complexity. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 106:467–82; reprinted (1969) in The sciences of the artificial, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [GS]Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1978) Universal and particular in the acquisition of language. Paper presented at the workshop-conference, “Language acquisition: state of the art,” University of Pennsylvania. 05 19–22. [RFC]Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1979) The role of language in language acquisition. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia, 04 21. [RFC]Google Scholar
Smedslund, J. (1961) The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children: III. Extinction of conservation of weight acquired “normally” and by means of empirical controls on a balance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 2:85–87. [RFC]Google Scholar
Smith, N. (1979) Syntax for psychologists. In: Morton, J., and Marshall, J. C. (eds.) Psycholinguistics Series. Vol. 2. London: Elek. [JCM]Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., and Ferguson, C. A. (eds.) (1977) Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. [RFC]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R., and Simmelhag, V. L. (1971) The “superstitution” experiment: a reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review 78:16–43. [HR]Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1978a) Empiricism, innateness and linguistic universals. Philosophical Studies 33:273–86. [SPS]Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1978b) Beliefs and subdoxastic states. Philosophy of Science 45:499–518. [SPS]Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1979) Between Chomskian rationalism and Popperian empiricism. British Journalfor the Philosophy of Science 30. [SPS]Google Scholar
Strohner, H., and Nelson, K. E. (1974) The young child's development of sentence comprehension: influence of event probability, nonverbal context, syntactic form, and strategies. Child Development 45:567–76. [RFC]Google Scholar
Taub, E. (1976) Motor behavior following deafferentation in the developmentally and motorically mature monkey. In: Herrman, R. M.; Grillner, S.; Stein, P. S. G.; and Stuart, D. G. (eds.) Neural control of locomotion. New York: Plenum. [NC]Google Scholar
Terrace, H. (forthcoming) Is problem solving language? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. [NC]Google Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. (1977) Contest and Cognition: Knowledge frames and speech act comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics 1:211–32. [JA]Google Scholar
Velletri-Glass, A.; Gazzaniga, M.; and Premack, D. (1973) Artificial language training in global aphasics. Neuropsychologia 11:95–104. [NC, RFC]Google Scholar
Walter, J. C. (1805) Etwas über Herrn Dr. Gall's Hlrnschädellehre. Berlin: Scherer. [JCM]Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1978) A principle theory for language acquisition, Social Science Research Reports, vol. 20, Univ. of California, Irvine. [NC]Google Scholar
Wiens, J. A. (1970) Effects of early experience on substrate pattern selection in Rana aurora tadpoles. Copeia No. 3, 543–48. [GG]Google Scholar
Wilcox, S., and Palermo, D. S. (1975) “In,” “on,” and “under” revisited. Cognition 3:245–54. [RFC]Google Scholar
Winograd, T. (1972) Understanding natural language. Cognitive Psychology 2:1–191. [SPS]Google Scholar
Winograd, T. (1976) Towards a procedural understanding of semantics. Revue Internationale de Philosophie 30:260–303. [GH]Google Scholar
Young, R. M. (1970) Mind, brain and adaptation in the nineteenth century. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [JCM]Google Scholar
Zangwill, O. L. (1978). Aphasia and the concept of brain centers. In: Miller, C. A., and Lenneberg, E. (eds.) Psychology and biology of language and thought. New York: Academic Press. [JM]Google Scholar
Zeki, S. M. (1978) Functional specialization in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey. Nature 274:423–28. [JCM]Google Scholar