Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T09:42:01.507Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evolutionary string theory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2007

Zen Faulkes
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, The University of Texas-Pan American, Edinburg, TX 78541. [email protected]://panam2.panam.edu/~zfaulkes/[email protected]://www.panam.edu/dept/biology/davelos5.html
Anita Davelos Baines
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, The University of Texas-Pan American, Edinburg, TX 78541. [email protected]://panam2.panam.edu/~zfaulkes/[email protected]://www.panam.edu/dept/biology/davelos5.html

Abstract

Evolution in Four Dimensions claims that epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic inheritance systems should be considered equal partners to genetics in evolutionary biology. The evidence for, and applicable scope of, these additional inheritance systems is limited, particularly with regard to areas involving learning. It is unclear how including these extra dimensions in mainstream evolutionary thinking translates into testable hypotheses for a productive research program.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baptista, L. F. & Trail, P. W. (1992) The role of song in the evolution of passerine diversity. Systematic Biology 41(2):242–47.Google Scholar
Belyaev, D. K., Ruvinsky, A. O. & Borodin, P. M. (1981a) Inheritance of alternative states of the fused gene in mice. Journal of Heredity 72(2):107–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belyaev, D. K., Ruvinsky, A. O. & Trut, L. N. (1981b) Inherited activation-inactivation of the star gene in foxes: Its bearing on the problem of domestication. Journal of Heredity 72(4):267–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boughman, J. W. (1998) Vocal learning by greater spear-nosed bats. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265(1392):227–33.Google Scholar
Breland, K. & Breland, M. (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist 16:681–84.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976) The selfish gene. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Deeckea, V. B., Ford, J. K. B. & Spong, P. (2000) Dialect change in resident killer whales: Implications for vocal learning and cultural transmission. Animal Behaviour 60(5):629–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gulevich, R. G., Oskina, I. N., Shikhevich, S. G., Fedorova, E. V. & Trut, L. N. (2004) Effect of selection for behavior on pituitary-adrenal axis and proopiomelanocortin gene expression in silver foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Physiology and Behavior 82:513–18.Google Scholar
Healy, S. D., de Kort, S.R. & Clayton, N. S. (2005) The hippocampus, spatial memory and food hoarding: A puzzle revisited. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20(1):1722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. J. (2005) Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kukekova, A. V., Trut, L. N., Oskina, I. N., Johnson, J. L., Temnykh, S. V., Kharlamova, A. V., Shepeleva, D. V., Gulievich, R. G., Shikhevich, S. G., Graphodatsky, A. S., Aguirre, G. D. & Acland, G. M. (2007) A meiotic linkage map of the silver fox, aligned and compared to the canine genome. Genome Research 17(3):387–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukekova, A. V., Trut, L. N., Oskina, I. N., Kharlamova, A. V., Shikhevich, S. G., Kirkness, E. F., Aguirre, G. D. & Acland, G. M. (2004) A marker set for construction of a genetic map of the silver fox (Vulpes vulpes). Journal of Heredity 95(3):185–94.Google Scholar
Lindberg, J., Björnerfeldt, S., Saetre, P., Svartberg, K., Seehuus, B., Bakken, M., Vilà, C. & Jazin, E. (2005) Selection for tameness has changed brain gene expression in silver foxes. Current Biology 15(22):915–26.Google Scholar
Morange, M. (2002) The relations between genetics and epigenetics: An historical point of view. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 981(1):5060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poole, J. H., Tyack, P. L., Stoeger-Horwath, A. S. & Watwood, S. (2005) Animal behaviour: Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature 434(7032):455–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trut, L. N. (1999) Early canid domestication: The farm-fox experiment. American Scientist 87(2):160–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, M. J. & Wells, J. (1957) The function of the brain of Octopus in tactile discrimination. Journal of Experimental Biology 34(1):131–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. & van Schaik, P.C. (2007) The evolution of animal “cultures” and social intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 362(1480):603–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, E. O. (1975) Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Wiley.Google Scholar
Woit, P. (2002) Is string theory even wrong? American Scientist 90(2):110–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar