Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T12:56:29.213Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2007

Thomas Suddendorf
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, [email protected]
Michael C. Corballis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New [email protected]

Abstract

In a dynamic world, mechanisms allowing prediction of future situations can provide a selective advantage. We suggest that memory systems differ in the degree of flexibility they offer for anticipatory behavior and put forward a corresponding taxonomy of prospection. The adaptive advantage of any memory system can only lie in what it contributes for future survival. The most flexible is episodic memory, which we suggest is part of a more general faculty of mental time travel that allows us not only to go back in time, but also to foresee, plan, and shape virtually any specific future event. We review comparative studies and find that, in spite of increased research in the area, there is as yet no convincing evidence for mental time travel in nonhuman animals. We submit that mental time travel is not an encapsulated cognitive system, but instead comprises several subsidiary mechanisms. A theater metaphor serves as an analogy for the kind of mechanisms required for effective mental time travel. We propose that future research should consider these mechanisms in addition to direct evidence of future-directed action. We maintain that the emergence of mental time travel in evolution was a crucial step towards our current success.

Type
Main Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albrecht, U. & Eichele, G. (2003) The mammalian circadian clock. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development 13(3):271–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, R. D. (1989) Evolution of the human psyche. In: The human revolution : Behavioral and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans, ed. Mellars, P. & Stringer, C., pp. 455531. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Allen, C. (2002) A skeptic's progress. Biology and Philosophy 17(5):695702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Schooler, L. J. (1991) Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science 2:396408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atance, C. M. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005) My future self: Young children's ability to anticipate and explain future states. Cognitive Development 20:341–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atance, C. M. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2006) Preschoolers' current desires warp their choices for the future. Psychological Science 17(7):583–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atance, C. M. & O'Neill, D. K. (2005) The emergence of episodic future thinking in humans. Learning and Motivation 36(2):126–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babb, S. J. & Crystal, J. D. (2005) Discrimination of what, when, and where: Implications for episodic-like memory in rats. Learning and Motivation 36:177–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1992) Working memory. Science 255:556–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bard, K., Todd, B. K., Bernier, C., Love, J. & Leavens, D. A. (2006) Self-awareness in human and chimpanzee infants: What is measured and what is meant by the mark and mirror test. Infancy 9:191219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L., Henzi, P. & Dunbar, R. (2003) Primate cognition: From “what now?” to “what if?” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(11):494–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (2004) Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered. Biology and Philosophy 19(2):283–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bischof, N. (1985) Das Rätzel Ödipus [The Oedipus riddle]. Piper.Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985) Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In: Emotion und Reflexivität, ed. Eckensberger, L. H. & Lantermann, E. D., pp. 347. Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Boaz, N. T., Ciochon, R. L., Xu, Q. & Liu, J. (2004) Mapping and taphonomic analysis of the Homo erectus loci at Locality 1 Zhoukoudian, China. Journal of Human Evolution 46(5):519–49. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WJS-4C5PTXB-1/2/4378ec51eb16b19e76acf42ee97fb6e1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browning, R. (1896) The poetical works. Smith, Elder & Co.Google Scholar
Bugnyar, T. & Heinrich, B. (2006) Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behavior to social context and identity of competitors. Animal Cognition 9:369–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, D. & Fulham, B. J. (2003) An evolved spatial memory bias in a nectar-feeding bird. Animal Behavior 66:695701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busby, J. & Suddendorf, T. (2005) Recalling yesterday and predicting tomorrow. Cognitive Development 20(3):362–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. (2001) Chimpanzee social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:388–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. (2004) Inferences about the location of food in the great apes (Pan pansicus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 118:232–41.Google Scholar
Call, J. (2005) The self and other: A missing link in comparative social cognition. In: The missing link in cognition, ed. Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., pp. 321–41. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J. & Carpenter, M. (2001) Do chimpanzees and children know what they have seen? Animal Cognition 4:207–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (1998) Chimpanzee gaze following in an object choice task. Animal Cognition 1:8999.Google Scholar
Calvin, W. H. (1982) Did throwing stones shape hominid brain evolution? Ethology and Sociobiology 3(3):115–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. & Dickinson, A. (2003) Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4:685–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, N. S. & Dickinson, A. (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395:272–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, N. S., Yu, K. S. & Dickinson, A. (2001) Scrub Jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) form integrated memories of the multiple features of caching episodes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 27:1729.Google ScholarPubMed
Collier-Baker, E. & Suddendorf, T. (2006) Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and 2-year-old children (Homo sapiens) understand double invisible displacement? Journal of Comparative Psychology 120:8997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, M., Singer, J. A. & Tagini, A. (2004) The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Social Cognition 22:491529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (1991) The lopsided ape. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2003) Recursion as the key to the human mind. In: From mating to mentality: Evaluating evolutionary psychology, ed. Sterelny, K. & Fitness, J., pp. 155–71. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2004) The origins of modernity: Was autonomous speech the critical factor? Psychological Review 111:543–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, M. C. (in press) Language, recursion, and starlings. Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. & Suddendorf, T. (in press) Memory, time and language. In: What makes us human?, ed. Pasternak, C., pp. 1736. Oneworld.Google Scholar
Dally, J. M., Clayton, N. S. & Emery, N. J. (2006a) The behavior and evolution of cache protection and pilferage. Animal Behavior 72:1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dally, J. M., Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N. S. (2006b) Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science 312(5780):1662–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Argembeau, A. & Van der Linden, M. (2004) Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition 13:844–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. (2000) An open letter to Prince Charles. Available at http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/prince/prince_index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1995) Darwin's dangerous idea. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Dere, E., Huston, J. P. & Silva, M. A. S. (2005) Episodic-like memory in mice: Simultaneous assessment of object, place and temporal order memory. Brain Research Protocols 16(1–3):1019.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. I. (1982) The informational character of representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5:376–77.Google Scholar
Dudai, Y. & Carruthers, M. (2005a) Memory: Some systems in the brain may be better equipped to handle the future than the past. Nature 434:567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003) The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 32:163–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eacott, M. J., Easton, A. & Zinkivskay, A. (2005) Recollection in an episodic-like memory task in the rat. Learning and Memory 12(3):221–23.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H., Fortin, N. J., Ergorul, C., Wright, S. P. & Agster, K. L. (2005) Episodic recollection in animals: “If it walks like a duck and quacks like a duck…” Learning and Motivation 36(2):190207.Google Scholar
Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N. S. (2001) Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414:443–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N. S. (2004) The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306:1903–907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etscorn, F. & Stephens, R. (1973) Establishment of conditioned taste aversions with a 24-hour CS-US interval. Physiological Psychology 1:251–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T. & Hauser, M. D. (2004) Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303:377–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C. & Ward, C. V. (2005) Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Behavior 26:1046.Google Scholar
Friedman, W. J. (1993) Memory for the time of past events. Psychological Bulletin 113(1):4466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, W. J. (2000) The development of children's knowledge of the times of future events. Child Development 71(4):913–32.Google Scholar
Friedman, W. J. (2005) Developmental and cognitive perspectives on humans' sense of the times of past and future events. Learning and Motivation 36:145–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, W. J. & Kemp, S. (1998) The effects of elapsed time and retrieval on young children's judgements of the temporal distances of past events. Cognitive Development 13:335–67.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1970) Chimpanzees: Self recognition. Science 167:8687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R. & Koelling, R. (1966) Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science 5:121–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J. & Koelling, R. (1966) Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 4:123–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardiner, J. M., Ramponi, C. & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2002) Recognition of memory and decision processes: A meta-analysis of remember, know, and guess responses. Memory 10(2):8398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, T. O., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D. & Nusbaum, H. C. (2006) Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440:1204–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, D. T. (2006) Stumbling on happiness. Knopf.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Gill, M. J. & Wilson, T. D. (2002) The future is now: Temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 88:430–44.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Slaughter, V. (1991) Young children's understanding of changes in their mental states. Child Development 62(1):98110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hailman, J. P. & Ficken, M. S. (1986) Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as “language” by structural linguistics. Animal Behavior 34:1899–901.Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1927) A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection, Part V: Selection and mutation. Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 23:838–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallos, J. (2005) “15 minutes of fame:” Exploring the temporal dimension of Middle Pleistocene lithic technology. Journal of Human Evolution 49:155–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. R. (2001) Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98(9):5359–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. R., Hampstead, B. M. & Murray, E. A. (2005) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) demonstrate robust memory for what and where, but not for when, in an open-field test of memory. Learning and Motivation 36:245–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, R. R. & Schwartz, B. L. (2004) Episodic memory in nonhumans: What, and where, is when? Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14:192–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B. & Tomasello, M. (2000) Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behavior 59:771–85.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2001) Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behavior 61:139–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harner, L. (1975) Yesterday and tomorrow: Development of early understanding of the terms. Developmental Psychology 11(6):864–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (2001) Folk physics for apes – The chimpanzee's theory of how the world works. Science 291(5503):440–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (2003) Knowing about knowing: Dissociations between perception and action systems over evolution and during development. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1001:79103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, W. T. (2002) The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–79.Google Scholar
Hazlitt, W. (1805) An essay on the principles of human action. J. Johnson.Google Scholar
Henderson, J., Hurly, T. A., Bateson, M. & Healy, S. D. (2006) Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus. Current Biology 16(5):512–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henson, R. N. A., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., Josephs, O. & Dolan, R. J. (1999) Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience 19:3962–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesslow, G. (2002) Conscious thought as simulation of behavior and perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(6):242–47.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:101–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holden, C. (2005) Time's up on time travel. Science 308:1110.Google Scholar
Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R. & Sosa, B. B. (1995) Planning in the real world: Preschool children's scripts and plans for familiar events. Child Development 66(4):984–98.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1976) The social function of intellect. In: Growing points in ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hunt, G. (1996) Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature 379:249–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. & Gray, R. (2003) Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B – Biological Sciences 270:867–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jerison, H. J. (1973) The evolution of the brain and intelligence. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kali, S. & Dayan, P. (2004) Off-line replay maintains declarative memories in a model of hippocampal-neocortical interaction. Nature Neuroscience 7:286–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenward, B., Weir, A. A. S., Rutz, C. & Kacelnik, A. (2005) Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature 433:121.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J. & Chance, S. (2002a) Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review 109:306–29.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J. & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002b) Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient's ability to remember the past and imagine the future. Social Cognition 20:353–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroodsma, D. E. & Momose, H. (1991) Songs of the Japanese population of the winter wren (Troglodytes troglodytes). The Condor 93:424–32.Google Scholar
Lea, S. E. G. (2001) Anticipation and memory as criteria for special welfare consideration. Animal Welfare 10(S):195208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leaver, L. A., Hopewell, L., Cadwell, C. & Mallarky, L. (in press) Audience effects on foodcaching in grey squirrels. Animal Cognition.Google Scholar
Levine, B. (2004) Autobiographical memory and the self in time: Brain lesion effects, functional neuroanatomy, and lifespan development. Brain and Cognition 55:5468.Google Scholar
Lewis, P. A. & Miall, R. C. (2006) Remembering the time: A continuous clock. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:401406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. F. & Ketcham, K. (1994) The myth of repressed memory. St. Martin's Press.Google Scholar
McKenzie, T. L. B., Bird, L. R. & Roberts, W. A. (2005) The effects of cache modification on food caching and retrieval behavior by rats. Learning and Motivation 36(2):260–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menzel, E. (2005) Progress in the study of chimpanzee recall and episodic memory. In: The missing link in cognition, ed. Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., pp. 188224. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mercado, E., Murray, S. O., Uyeyama, R. K., Pack, A. A. & Herman, L. M. (1998) Memory for recent actions in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Repetition of arbitrary behaviors using an abstract rule. Animal Learning and Behavior 26(2):210–18.Google Scholar
Mischel, H. N. & Mischel, W. (1983) The development of children's knowledge of self-control strategies. Child Development 54:603–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyashita, Y. (2004) Cognitive memory: Cellular and network machineries and their top-down control. Science 306:435–40.Google Scholar
Moore, C., Barresi, J. & Thompson, C. (1998) The cognitive basis of future-oriented prosocial behavior. Social Development 7(2):198218.Google Scholar
Mulcahy, N. J. & Call, J. (2006) Apes save tools for future use. Science 312:1038–40.Google Scholar
Mulcahy, N. J., Call, J. & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2005) Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. Journal of Comparative Psychology 119:2332.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Collier-Baker, E., Davis, J. & Suddendorf, T. (2005) Imitation recognition in a captive chimpanzee (Pan Troglodytes). Animal Cognition 8:3136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M., Suddendorf, T. & Slaughter, V. (2006) Self-recognition beyond the face. Child Development 77:176–85.Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. K. & Feldman, M. W. L. (2003) Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J. P. (2004) Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14:769–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Doherty, J. P., Deichmann, R., Critchley, H. D. & Dolan, R. J. (2002) Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron 33(5):815–26.Google Scholar
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., Kawashima, R., Fukuda, H., Itoh, M. & Yamadori, A. (2003) Thinking of the future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage 19:1369–80.Google Scholar
Osvath, M. & Gärdenfors, P. (2005) Oldowan culture and the evolution of anticipatory cognition. Lund University Cognitive Studies 122:116.Google Scholar
Patterson, F. & Cohen, R. H. (1994) Self-recognition and self-awareness in lowland gorillas. In: Self-awareness in animal and humans, ed. Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. & Boccia, M. L., pp. 273–90. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J. (1991) Understanding the representational mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2003) Language as an adaptation to the cognitive niche. In: Language evolution, ed. Christiansen, M. H. & Kirby, S., pp. 1637. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. (2000) Folk physics for apes. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Vonk, J. (2003) Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:157–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:515–26.Google Scholar
Roberts, W. A. (2002) Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin 128(3):473–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H. L. III & McDermott, K. B. (1995) Creating false memories remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 21:803–14.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C. & Schulkind, M. D. (1997) Distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults. Psychology and Aging 12(3):524–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L. (1996) Searching for memory: The brain, the mind, and the past. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Scheumann, M. & Call, J. (2006) Sumatran orangutans and a yellow-cheeked crested gibbon know what is where. International Journal of Primatology 27(2):575602.Google Scholar
Schoenemann, P. T., Sheenan, M. J. & Glotzer, L. D. (2005) Prefrontal white matter volume is disproportionately larger in humans than in other primates. Nature Neuroscience 8:242–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (2006) Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annual Review of Psychology 57:87115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, B. L. & Evans, S. (2001) Episodic memory in primates. American Journal of Primatology 55(2):7185.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. L., Colon, M. R., Sanchez, I. C., Rodriguez, I. A. & Evans, S. (2002) Single-trial learning of “what” and “who” information in a gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): Implications for episodic memory. Animal Cognition 5(2):8590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. L., Hoffman, M. L. & Evans, S. (2005) Episodic-like memory in a gorilla: A review and new findings. Learning and Motivation 36(2):226–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K. & van Hoesen, G. W. (1998) Limbic frontal cortex in hominoids: A comparative study of area 13. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 106:129–55.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K. & van Hoesen, G. W. (2001) Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 114:224–41.Google Scholar
Semendeferi, K., Damasio, H. & Frank, R. (1997) The evolution of the frontal lobes: A volumetric analysis based on three-dimensional reconstructions of magnetic resonance scans of human and ape brains. Journal of Human Evolution 32:375–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Bergman, T. J. (2005) Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:264–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shettleworth, S. J. & Sutton, J. E. (2006) Do animals know what they know? In: Rational animals?, ed. Hurley, S. & Nudds, M., pp. 235–46. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. C. & Reichman, O. J. (1984) The evolution of food caching by birds and mammals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15:329–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J. D., Shields, W. E. & Washburn, D. A. (2003) The comparative psychology of uncertainty monitoring and metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26(3):317.Google Scholar
Son, L. K. & Kornell, N. (2005) Metaconfidence judgements in rhesus macaques: Explicit versus implicit mechanisms. In: The missing link in cognition, ed. Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., pp. 296320. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N. & Levine, B. (2006) The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan. Memory and Cognition 34:1644–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1992) Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4(3):232–43.Google Scholar
Steele, M. A., Turner, G., Smallwood, P. D., Wolff, J. O. & Radillo, J. (2001) Cache management by small mammals: Experimental evidence for the significance of acorn-embryo excision. Journal of Mammalogy 82(1):3542.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2003) Thought in a hostile world. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Suarez, S. & Gallup, G. G. (1981) Self recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas. Journal of Human Evolution 10:175–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (1994) Discovery of the fourth dimension: Mental time travel and human evolution. Unpublished Masters thesis, University of Waikato, Hamilton. Available at: http://cogprints.org/729/Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (1999) The rise of the metamind. In: The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution, ed. Corballis, M. C. & ea, S. E. G., pp. 218–60. Oxford University Press. Available at: http://cogprints.org/732/Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2004) How primatology can inform us about the evolution of the human mind. Australian Psychologist 39(3):180–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2006) Foresight and evolution of the human mind. Science 312(5776):10061007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T. & Busby, J. (2003a) Like it or not? The mental time travel debate. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:437–38.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Busby, J. (2003b) Mental time travel in animals? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(9):391396Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Busby, J. (2005) Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation 36(2):110–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (1997) Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs 123(2):133–67. Available at: http://cogprints.org/725/Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Whiten, A. (2001) Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes and other animals. Psychological Bulletin 127(5):629–50.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Whiten, A. (2003) Reinterpreting the mentality of apes. In: From mating to mentality: Evaluating evolutionary psychology, ed. Sterelny, K. & Fitness, J., pp 173–96. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Szagun, G. (1978) On the frequency of use of tenses in English and German children's spontaneous speech. Child Development 49:898901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanaka, S. C., Doya, K., Okada, G., Ueda, K., Okamoto, Y. & Yamawaki, S. (2004) Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nature Neuroscience 7:887–93.Google Scholar
Thieme, H. (1997) Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature 385(6619):807–10.Google Scholar
Thompson, C., Barresi, J. & Moore, C. (1997) The development of future-oriented prudence and altruism in preschoolers. Cognitive Development 12:199212.Google Scholar
Thompson, N. S. (1969) Individual identification and temporal patterning in the cawing of common crows. Communications in Behavioral Biology 4:2933.Google Scholar
Thorpe, C. M., Jacova, C. & Wilkie, D. M. (2004) Some pitfalls in measuring memory in animals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28(7):711–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. (2003) Chimpanzees understand psychological states – The question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:53156.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28:675735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, J. & DeVore, I. (1987) The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modelling. In: The evolution of human behavior: Primate models, ed. Kinzey, W., pp. 183238. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Organization of memory, ed. Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W., pp. 382403. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1984) Précis of Elements of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(2):223–68.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985) Memory and consciousness. Canadian Journal of Psychology 26:112.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2005) Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In: The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness, ed. Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., pp. 356. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uylings, H. B. M. (1990) The prefrontal cortex: Its structure, function, and pathology. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. (2001) Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72:655–84.Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T. & Tulving, E. (1997) Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin 121(3):331–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (1998) Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 112:270–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2001) Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee's theory of how the world works. Nature 409(6817):133.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Byrne, R. W. (1988) Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11:233–73.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Suddendorf, T. (2001) Meta-representation and secondary representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:378.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Suddendorf, T. (in press) Great ape cognition and the evolutionary roots of human imagination. In: Imaginative minds, ed. Roth, I.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., Rose, G. & MacLeod, A. K. (1996) The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future Memory and Cognition 24:116–25.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. A. & McNaughton, B. L. (1994) Reactivation of Hippocampal Ensemble Memories During Sleep. Science 265(5172):676–79.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D. & Gilbert, D. T. (2005) Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14:131–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, B. & Collard, M. (1999) Anthropology – The human genus. Science 284(5411):6571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zentall, T. R. (2006) Mental time travel in animals: A challenging question. Behavioural Processes 72:173–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zentall, T. R., Clement, T. S., Bhatt, R. S. & Allen, J. (2001) Episodic-like memory in pigeons. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8(4):685–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed