Article contents
Environmentally invoked innovation and cognition
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 17 December 2007
Abstract
Behavioral innovations induced by the social or physical environment are likely to be of great functional and evolutionary importance, and thus warrant serious attention. Innovation provides a process by which animals can adjust to changed environments. Despite this apparent adaptive advantage, it is not known whether innovative propensities are adaptive specializations. Furthermore, the varied psychological processes underlying innovation remain poorly understood.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007
References
Bateson, P. P. G. (1983) Genes, environment and the development of behaviour. In: Animal behaviour, vol. 3: Genes, development and learning, ed. Halliday, T. R. & Slater, P. J. B., pp. 52–81. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2003) Novelty in deceit. In: Animal innovation, ed. Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N., pp. 237–59. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, G. & Crozier, R. H. (2003) A flexible theory of evolution. Nature 424:16–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewar, G. (2004) Social and asocial cues about new food: Cue reliability influences intake in rats. Learning and Behavior 32:82–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. M. & Visalberghi, E. (1990) Social processes affecting the appearance of innovative behaviours in capuchin monkeys. Folia Primatologica 54:155–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D. (1988) Invention and social transmission: New data from wild vervet monkeys. In: Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans, ed. Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A., pp. 327–43. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kothbauer-Hellman, R. (1990) On the origin of a tradition: Milk bottle opening by titmice. Zoologischer Anzeiger 225:353–61.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Janik, V. (2006) The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21(10):542–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, P. (1991) Adaptations to environmental change: An evolutionary perspective. In: Primate responses to environmental change, ed. Box, H. O., pp. 39–56. Chapman & Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M. & Webster, S. J. (2001) Novel food use by Gray Kingbirds and Red-necked Pigeons in Barbados. Bulletin of the British Ornithologists' Club 121:247–49.Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E. & Finkelstein, A. (1997) Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour 53:549–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, P. T., Réale, D., Sol, D. & Reader, S. M. (2006) Wildlife conservation and animal temperament: Causes and consequences of evolutionary change for captive, reintroduced, and wild populations. Animal Conservation 9:39–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morand-Ferron, J., Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M., Sol, D. & Elvin, S. (2004) Dunking behaviour in Carib grackles. Animal Behaviour 68:1267–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfeffer, K., Fritz, J. & Kotrschal, K. (2002) Hormonal correlates of being an innovative greylag goose, Anser anser. Animal Behaviour 63:687–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M. (2004) Distinguishing social and asocial learning using diffusion dynamics. Learning & Behavior 32:90–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (2001) Primate innovation: Sex, age and social rank differences. International Journal of Primatology 22:787–805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M. (2002) Social intelligence, innovation and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99:4436–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S. M. (2003a) Animal innovation: An introduction. In: Animal innovation, ed. Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N., pp. 3–35. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M., Morand-Ferron, J., Côté, I. & Lefebvre, L. (2002a) Unusual feeding behaviors in five species of Barbadian birds. El Pitirre 15:117–23.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., Nover, D. & Lefebvre, L. (2002b) Locale-specific sugar packet opening by Lesser Antillean bullfinches in Barbados. Journal of Field Ornithology 73:82–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews 82:291–318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlichting, C. D. & Pigliucci, M. (1998) Phenotypic evolution: A reaction norm perspective. Sinauer.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Galef, B. G. Jr. (1984) Cultural transmission without imitation: Milk bottle opening by birds. Animal Behaviour 32(3):937–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Galef, B. G. Jr. (1990) Social learning without imitation: More about milk bottle opening by birds. Animal Behaviour 40(5):987–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sih, A., Bell, A. M., Johnson, J. C. & Ziemba, R. E. (2004) Behavioral syndromes: An integrative overview. Quarterly Review of Biology 79:241–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sol, D. (2003) Behavioral innovation: A neglected issue in the ecological and evolutionary literature? In: Animal innovation, ed. Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N., pp. 63–82. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1990) Food-washing behaviour in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, and crabeating macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Animal Behaviour 40:829–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyles, J. S., Kundel, J. G. & Wilson, A. C. (1983) Birds, behaviour, and anatomical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 80:4394–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
- 7
- Cited by