Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T09:09:08.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early development of body representations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2007

Tamara Christie
Affiliation:
Early Cognitive Development Unit, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia. [email protected]://www.psy.uq.edu.au/people/[email protected]://www.psy.uq.edu.au/people/personal.html?id=35
Virginia Slaughter
Affiliation:
Early Cognitive Development Unit, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia. [email protected]://www.psy.uq.edu.au/people/[email protected]://www.psy.uq.edu.au/people/personal.html?id=35

Abstract

The dissociations among body representations that Dijkerman & de Haan (D&dH) describe are also supported by developmental evidence. Developmental dissociations among different types of body-related representations suggest distinct functional systems from the start, rather than progressive differentiation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anisfeld, M. (1996) Only tongue protrusion modelling is matched by neonates. Developmental Review 16:149–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L. E. & Watson, J. S. (1985) Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology 21:963–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourgeois, K., Khawar, A., Neal, S. A. & Lockman, J. (2005) Infant manual exploration of objects, surfaces, and their interrelations. Infancy 8:233–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. E. & Hopkins, B. (1988) Hand-mouth coordination in the newborn human infant. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 6:303–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. & Meltzoff, A. (1996) The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology 9:213–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew, A. & Butterworth, G. E. (1995) Hand-mouth contact in newborn babies before and after feeding. Developmental Psychology 31:456–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. & Moore, K. (1995) Infants' understanding of people and things: From body imitation to folk psychology. In: The body and the self, ed. Bermúdez, J., Marcel, A. & Eilan, N., pp. 4369. MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. & Takeshita, H. (2006) Do human fetuses anticipate self-oriented actions? A study by four-dimensional (4D) ultrasonography. Infancy 10:289301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, C., Atkinson, J. & Braddick, O. (2001) The development of reaching and looking preferences in infants to objects of different sizes. Developmental Psychology 37:561–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1953) The origins of intelligence in the child. Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Reed, C. L. (2002) What is the body schema? In: The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases, ed. Meltzoff, A. N. & Prinz, W., pp. 233–43. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K. & Sunderland, T. (1991) Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing: Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia. Brain 114:629–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slaughter, V. & Heron, M. (2004) Origins and early development of human body knowledge. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial no. 276, Whole no. 69(2).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witt, A., Cermak, S. & Coster, W. (1990) Body part identification in 1- to 2-year-old children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 44:147–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed