Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T09:12:29.693Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conceptual discontinuity involves recycling old processes in new domains

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2011

David Landy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Richmond, Richmond, VA 23173. [email protected]/~dlandy
Colin Allen
Affiliation:
Cognitive Science Program and Department of History & Philosophy of Science, Indiana University, Bloomington IN 47405. [email protected]://www.agcognition.org
Michael L. Anderson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Franklin & Marshall College, Lancaster, PA 17603. [email protected]

Abstract

We dispute Carey's assumption that distinct core cognitive processes employ domain-specific input analyzers to construct proprietary representations. We give reasons to believe that conceptual systems co-opt core components for new domains. Domain boundaries, as well as boundaries between perceptual–motor and conceptual cognitive resources may be useful abstractions, but do not appear to reflect constraints respected by brains and cognitive systems.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. L. (2010) Neural re-use as a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(4):245–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andres, M., Seron, X. & Oliver, E. (2007) Contribution of hand motor circuits to counting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:563–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badets, A. & Pesenti, M. (2010) Creating number semantics through finger movement perception. Cognition 115(1):4653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (2009) The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2008) Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörfler, W. (2004) Diagrams as means and objects of mathematical reasoning. In: Developments in mathematics education in German-speaking countries. Selected papers from the annual conference on didactics of mathematics 2001, ed. Weigand, H.-G., pp. 3949. Verlag Franzbecker.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003) Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P. & Dehaene, S. (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6(6):435–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirshner, D. (1989) The visual syntax of algebra. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 20(3):274–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirshner, D. & Awtry, T. (2004) Visual salience of algebraic transformations. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 35(4): 224–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landy, D. & Goldstone, R. L. (2007) How abstract is symbolic thought? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(4):720–33.Google ScholarPubMed
Landy, D., Jones, M. N. & Goldstone, R. L. (2008) How the appearance of an operator affects its formal precedence. In: Proceedings of the Thirtieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Love, B. C., McRae, K. & Sloutsky, V. M., pp. 2109–14. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Longo, M. R. & Lourenco, S. F. (2007) Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia 45(7):14004007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longo, M. R. & Lourenco, S. F. (2009) Bisecting the mental number line in near and far space. Brain and Cognition 72(3):362–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed