Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T06:01:34.284Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A complete theory of psychosis and autism as diametric disorders of social brain must consider full range of clinical syndromes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 June 2008

Katharine N. Thakkar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240. [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] URL: http://www.psy.vanderbilt.edu/faculty/sohee/
Natasha Matthews
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240. [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] URL: http://www.psy.vanderbilt.edu/faculty/sohee/
Sohee Park
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240. [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] URL: http://www.psy.vanderbilt.edu/faculty/sohee/

Abstract

We argue that autism and psychosis spectrum disorders cannot be conceptualized as polar extremes of mentalizing ability. We raise two main objections: (1) the autistic-psychotic continuum, as conceptualized by the authors, excludes defining features of schizophrenia spectrum: negative symptoms, which correlate more strongly with mentalizing impairments; and (2) little evidence exists for a relationship between mentalizing ability and positive symptoms.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995) Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brüne, M. (2005) “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31(1):2142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D. (2007) Suspicious minds: The psychology of persecutory delusions. Clinical Psychology Review 27(4):425–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. (1992) The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Psychology Press/Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hardy-Baylé, M. C. (1994) Organisation de l'action, phénomènes de conscience et représentation mentale de l'action chez des schizophrènes [Organization of action; the phenomenon of consciousness and mental representation of action in schizophrenia]. Actualités Psychiatriques 20:393400.Google Scholar
Hardy-Baylé, M. C., Sarfati, Y. & Passerieux, C. (2003) The cognitive basis of disorganization symptomatology in schizophrenia and its clinical correlates: Toward a pathogenetic approach to disorganization. Schizophrenia Bulletin 29:459–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, L., Siegert, R. J. & McClure, J. (2005b) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: A critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 10:249–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langdon, R. & Coltheart, M. (1999) Mentalising, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. Cognition 71:4371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walston, F., Blennerhassett, R. C. & Charlton, B. G. (2000) Theory of mind, persecutory delusions and the somatic marker mechanism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 5:161–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar