No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Commentary on creativity and curiosity
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 21 May 2024
Abstract
The target article covers a large amount of ground and offers a provocative perspective. This commentary focuses on (a) assumptions, namely that there are discrete stages in the creative process and that novelty and usefulness are inextricable, (b) hidden variables in the creativity–curiosity relationship, and (c) the difference between an explanation of creativity versus a description of influence on it.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Amabile, T. M. (1990). Within you, without you: The social psychology of creativity, and beyond. In Runco, M. A. & Albert, R. S. (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp. 61–91). Sage.Google Scholar
Berg, M. (2014). Commerce and creativity in eighteenth-century Birmingham. In Markets and manufacture in early industrial Europe (pp. 173–201). Routledge.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jesus, S. N., Rus, S. L., Lens, W., & Imaginário, S. (2013) Intrinsic motivation and creativity related to product: A meta-analysis of the studies published between 1990–2010. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 80–84, doi:10.1080/10400419.2013.752235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litchfield, R. C. (2008). Brainstorming reconsidered: A goal-based view. Academy of Management Review, 33, 649–668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. (1965). Personality and the realization of creative potential. American Psychologist, 20, 273–281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumford, M. D., & Gustafson, S. B. (1988). Creativity syndrome: Integration, application, and innovation. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 27–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, R. (Ed.). (2007). Everyday creativity and new views of human nature: Psychological, social, and spiritual perspectives. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rickards, T., & deCock, C. (2003). Understanding organizational creativity: Towards a multi-paradigmatic approach. In Runco, M. A. (Ed.), Creativity research handbook (Vol. 2, pp. 1–31). Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (1994). Conclusions concerning problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 272–290). Ablex.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2010). Creative thinking may be simultaneous as well as blind. Physics of Life Reviews, 7, 184–185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runco, M. A. (2019). Creativity as a dynamic, personal, parsimonious process. In Beghetto, R. & Corazza, G. (Eds.), Dynamic perspectives on creativity (pp. 181–188). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A. (2023). AI can only produce artificial creativity. Journal of Creativity, 33, 100063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 92–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaeffer, R., Miranda, B., & Koyejo, S. (2023). Are emergent abilities of large language models a mirage? arXiv:2304.15004v2 [cs.AI] https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2304.15004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2023). The blind-variation and selective-retention theory of creativity: Recent developments and current status of BVSR. Creativity Research Journal, 35, 304–323. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2059919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
A shared novelty-seeking basis for creativity and curiosity
Related commentaries (29)
A developmental account of curiosity and creativity
A shared “optimal-level of arousal”: Seeking basis for creativity and curiosity
An extension of the novelty-seeking model: Considering the plurality of novelty types and their differential interactions with memory
Be curious: Strategic curiosity drives creativity
Beyond novelty: Learnability in the interplay between creativity, curiosity and artistic endeavours
Breaking down (and moving beyond) novelty as a trigger of curiosity
Commentary on creativity and curiosity
Computational models of intrinsic motivation for curiosity and creativity
Creativity is motivated by novelty. Curiosity is triggered by uncertainty
Curiosity is more than novelty seeking
Curious? The relationship between curiosity and creativity is likely NOT novelty
Dissecting the neuroanatomy of creativity and curiosity: The subdivisions within networks matter
Distinct neurocognitive pathways underlying creativity: An integrative approach
Expanding horizons in reinforcement learning for curious exploration and creative planning
Exploratory exploitation and exploitative exploration: The phenomenology of play and the computational dynamics of search
Getting curiouser and curiouser about creativity: The search for a nuanced model
Ignoring the role of reiterative processing and worldview transformation leads to exaggeration of the role of curiosity in creativity
Is a wandering mind a novelty-seeking mind? The curious case of incubation
Mindfulness, curiosity, and creativity
Models need mechanisms, but not labels
Mood regulation as a shared basis for creativity and curiosity
Novelty seeking is neither necessary nor sufficient for curiosity or creativity, instead both curiosity and creativity may reflect an epistemic drive
Novelty seeking might underlie curiosity and the novelty dimension of creativity, but not the usefulness dimension
On the dual nature of creativity: Same same but different?
Prediction error minimization as a common computational principle for curiosity and creativity
Question-asking as a mechanism of information seeking
The costs of curiosity and creativity: Minimizing the downsides while maximizing the upsides
The creativity of architects
Toward a causal model of curiosity and creativity
Author response
A shared novelty-seeking basis for creativity and curiosity: Response to the commentators