Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T21:18:23.901Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Automatic (spontaneous) propositional and associative learning of first impressions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2009

James S. Uleman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003. [email protected]://www.psych.nyu.edu/uleman

Abstract

Contrary to the target article's claims, social cognition research shows considerable learning (about other people) that is relatively automatic. Some of this learning is propositional (spontaneous trait inferences) and some is associative (spontaneous trait transference). Other dichotomies – for example, between learning explicit and implicit attitudes – are also important. However conceived, human conditioning is not synonymous with human learning.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bargh, J. A. (1994) The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In: Handbook of social cognition, vol. 1, 2nd edition, ed. Wyer, R. S. Jr. & Srull, T. K., pp. 140. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlston, D. E. & Skowronski, J. J. (1994) Savings in the relearning of trait information as evidence for spontaneous inference generation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66:840–56.Google Scholar
Carlston, D. E. & Skowronski, J. J. (2005) Linking versus thinking: Evidence for the differential associative and attributional bases of spontaneous trait transference and spontaneous trait inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89:884–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, M. T., Skowronski, J. J., Stiff, C. & Scherer, C. R. (2007) Interfering with inferential, but not associative, processes underlying spontaneous trait inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33:677–90.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E. & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998) Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74:1464–80.Google Scholar
Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Mackie, D. M. & Strain, L. M. (2006) Of two minds: Forming and changing valence-inconsistent implicit and explicit attitudes. Psychological Science 17:954–58.Google Scholar
Skowronski, J. J., Carlston, D. E., Mae, L. & Crawford, M. T. (1998) Spontaneous trait transference: Communicators take on the qualities they describe in others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74:837–48.Google Scholar
Todorov, A. & Uleman, J. S. (2003) The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actors' faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39:549–62.Google Scholar
Todorov, A. & Uleman, J. S. (2004) The person reference process in spontaneous trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87:482–93.Google Scholar
Uleman, J. S., Hon, A., Roman, R. & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996a) On-line evidence for spontaneous trait inferences at encoding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22:377–94.Google Scholar
Uleman, J. S., Newman, L. S. & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996b) People as flexible interpreters: Evidence and issues from Spontaneous Trait Inference. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 28, ed. Zanna, M. P., pp. 211–79. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Uleman, J. S., Saribay, S. A. & Gonzalez, C. (2008) Spontaneous inferences, implicit impressions, and implicit theories. Annual Review of Psychology 59:329–60.Google Scholar
Zárate, M. A., Uleman, J. S. & Voils, C. I. (2001) Effects of culture and processing goals on the activation and binding of trait concepts. Social Cognition 19:295323.Google Scholar