Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T10:38:22.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Abstract or not? Insights from priming

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2009

Bert Reynvoet
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. [email protected]@psy.kuleuven.behttp://ppw.kuleuven.be/labexppsy/newSite/
Karolien Notebaert
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. [email protected]@psy.kuleuven.behttp://ppw.kuleuven.be/labexppsy/newSite/

Abstract

Cohen Kadosh & Walsh (CK&W) argue that numerical representation is primarily non-abstract. However, in their target article they failed to consider recent behavioral priming experiments. These priming experiments provide evidence for an abstract numerical representation under automatic conditions.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brysbaert, M. (2005) Number recognition in different formats. In: Handbook of mathematical cognition, ed. Campbell, J. I. D.. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. & Akhavein, R. (1995) Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21(2):314–26.Google Scholar
Den Heyer, K. & Briant, K. (1986) Priming single digit numbers: Automatic spreading activation dissipates as a function of semantic distance. American Journal of Psychology 99:315–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., Depestel, I., Fias, W. & Reynvoet, B. (2008) Cross-lingual numerical distance priming with L2 number words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61:1281–90.Google Scholar
Fias, W. (2001) Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychological Research – Psychologische Forschung 65(4):242–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hung, Y.-H., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J.-L. & Wu, D. H. (2008) Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers. Cognition 106(3):1441–50.Google Scholar
Ito, Y. & Hatta, T. (2004) Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition 32(4):662–73.Google Scholar
Koechlin, E., Naccache, L., Block, E. & Dehaene, S. (1999) Primed numbers: Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25(6):1882–905.Google Scholar
Naccache, L. & Dehaene, S. (2001b) Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition 80:223–37.Google Scholar
Reynvoet, B. & Brysbaert, M. (1999) Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line. Cognition 72:191201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynvoet, B. & Brysbaert, M. (2004) Cross-notation number priming investigated at different stimulus onset asynchronies in parity and naming tasks. Experimental Psychology 51(2):8190.Google Scholar
Reynvoet, B., Brysbaert, M. & Fias, W. (2002) Semantic priming in number naming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 55(4):1127–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B. & Verguts, T. (2005a) How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, & Hoffmann (2003) Cognition 97:8997.Google Scholar