Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T16:23:12.733Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social Interaction and Acceptance of Mildly Retarded Children in a Mainstream Special Educational Setting1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2016

Debbie Espiner
Affiliation:
Auckland Teachers College
Keri Wilton
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Ted Glynn
Affiliation:
University of Otago

Abstract

The social acceptance (by non-handicapped children) and classroom and playground social behaviour of the five former special class pupils was studied. Six months prior to the study the pupils had been transferred to an experimental mainstream special educational programme within the same school. At the time of the study, the mainstreamed children were all maintaining or surpassing the rates of academic progress they had shown in the special class and they were well accepted by their classmates. Their social behaviour during unstructured classroom and playground activities closely approximated that of their classmates, but in the case of playground activities differed markedly from that of pupils who had remained in the special class. The results indicate that mainstream special educational facilities can work effectively — i.e., academically and socially, for at least some mildly retarded children. Where resources permit and the support of regular class teachers is available, there would appear to be some advantages for the children concerned.

Type
Research and Review
Copyright
Copyright © The Australian Association of Special Education 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Carlberg, C. & Kavale, K. (1980) The efficacy of special versus regular class placement for exceptional children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Special Education, 14, 295309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corman, L. & Gottlieb, J. (1978) Mainstreaming mentally retarded children: A review of research. In Ellis, N.R. (Ed.) International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 9, pp. 251275). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Elley, W.B. & Irving, J.C. (1976) Revised socioeconomic index for New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 11, 2536.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, J. (1981) Mainstreaming: Fulfilling the promise. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86, 115126.Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, R.L., Gottlieb, J., Guskin, S., & Yoshida, R.K. (1978) Evaluating mainstreaming programs: Models, caveats, considerations, and guidelines. Exceptional Children, 44, 588601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parten, M.B. & Newhall, S.M. (1943) Social behaviour of preschool children. In Barker, R.G., Kounin, J.S., & Wright, H.F. (Eds.) Child behavior and development. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Robinson, N.M. & Robinson, H.B. (1976) The mentally retarded child: A psychological approach (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Semmel, M.I., Gottlieb, J., & Robinson, N.M. (1979) Mainstreaming: Perspectives on educating handicapped children in the public school. In Berliner, D. (Ed.) Review of research in education (Vol. 7). Washington, D.C.: American Educational Research Association.Google Scholar
Smart, R.F. & Wilton, K.M. (1975) Social participation and special class attendance in mildly retarded children. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 10, 6674.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. & Muenchow, S. (1979) Mainstreaming: The proof is in the implementation. American Psychologist, 34, 993996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed