Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T02:52:52.490Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing Morphological Development Within Special Education Settings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2016

Judith A. Bowey*
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Dolour M. Mathews
Affiliation:
Enoggera Special Education Unit, Queensland
*
Correspondence regarding this paper should be directed to Dr J.A. Bowey, Department of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4067, Australia.

Abstract

This study was designed both to evaluate the Biks and Gutches test as a measure of morphological development in children within special education settings, and to determine whether or not these children have acquired a generative morphology. Two groups of children were pretested on the Biks and Gutches test. An experimental group received a short teaching program designed to familiarize them with concepts underlying the Berko procedure. Both the experimental and the control group were then retested on the Biks and Gutches test. The teaching program enhanced performance, indicating that this test might in part reflect abilities other than morphological development. Children experienced greater difficulty with nonsense word than with real word items. However, performance on comparable real word and nonsense word items was strongly correlated, indicating that these children have acquired a generative morphology.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Australian Association of Special Education 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R.W. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clay, M.M., Gill, M., Glynn, T., McNaughton, T., & Salmon, K. (1983). Recordoforal language and Biks and Gutches. Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Dever, R.B. (1972). A comparison of the results of a revised version of Berko’s test of morphology with the free speech of mentally retarded children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 169178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dever, R.B., & Gardner, W.I. (1970). Performance of normal and retarded boys on Berko’s test of morphology. Language and Speech, 13, 162181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. Glasgow: Collins.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L.M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. (3rd ed.) Pine Circles, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Kirk, S.A., McCarthy, J.J., & Kirk, W.D. (1968). The Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Levy, Y. (1987). The Wug technique revisited. Cognitive Development, 2, 7187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovell, K., & Bradbury, B. (1967). The learning of English morphology in educationally subnormal special school children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 71, 609614.Google ScholarPubMed
McLeskey, J., & Rieth, H.J. (1982). Controlling IQ differences between reading disabled and normal children: An empirical example. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 15, 481483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newfield, M.U., & Schlanger, B.B. (1968). The acquisition of English morphology by normal and educable mentally retarded children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 11, 693706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, S. (1982). The language of the mentally retarded. In Rosenberg, S. (Ed.), Handbook of Applied Psycholiguistics. (pp. 329392). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, S. (1984). Disorders of first-language development: Trends in research and theory. In Gollin, E.S. (Ed.), Malformations of development: Biological and psychological sources and consequences, (pp. 195237). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, S., & Abbeduto, L. (1987). Indicators of linguistic competence in the peer group conversational behavior of mildly retarded adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 1932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, J. (1977). The silence of stupidity. In Morton, J. & Marshall, J.C. (Eds), Psycholinguistics: Developmental and Pathological, (pp. 99134). London: Paul Elek.Google Scholar
Storandt, M., & Hudson, W. (1975). Misuse of covariance in aging research and some partial solutions. Experimental Aging Research, 1, 121125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed