Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T14:29:50.544Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Transfer and developmental processes in adult foreign language speech production

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

James Emil Flege*
Affiliation:
University of Alabama in Birmingham
Richard D. Davidian
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
*
James E. Flege, Department of Biocommunication, University of Alabama in Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35296

Abstract

This study tested the hypothesis that factors that shape children’s production of their native language (L1) will also influence adults’ pronunciation of sounds in a foreign language (L2). The final stops in CVC English words produced by 12 adult native speakers each of English, Polish, Spanish, and Chinese were phonetically transcribed. The frequency with which these stops were devoiced, deleted, or fricativized was tabulated. The Spanish subjects (unlike the Chinese or Poles) showed the effect of a transfer process, producing word-final /b, d, g/as fricatives. Subjects in all three non-native groups (but not the native English subjects) resembled English-learning children in devoicing word-final /b, d, g/. Subjects whose L1 does not have word-final stops (i.e., the native speakers of Chinese and Spanish) showed another process commonly observed in English L1 acquisition: final stop deletion. A number of language background variables (e.g., age of arrival in the United States) were found not to be significantly correlated with the accuracy of final stop production. These findings suggest that, in addition to transfer processes arising from L1/L2 phonetic and phonological differences, developmental processes similar to those affecting child L1 speech production also influence adult L2 speech production.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Brady, J. S. Topics in Polish phonology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, 1975.Google Scholar
Burling, R. Social constraints on adult language learning. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Native language and foreign language acquisition. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 1981, 379, 279–90.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A., Yeni-Komshian, G., Zurif, E., & Carbone, E.The acquisition of a new phonological contrast: The case of stop consonants in French-English bilinguals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1973, 54, 421–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, C.-C.A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese. Paris: Mouton, 1973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donegan, P., & Stampe, D. The study of natural phonology. In Dinnsen, D. (Ed.), Current approaches to phonological theory. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Eckman, F. R.On the naturalness of interlanguage phonological rules. Language Learning, 1981, 31, 195216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, M. L.Word position in fricative acquisition. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 1979, 16, 6776.Google Scholar
Elsendoorn, B. A. G. Quality and quantity in English by Dutchman: Two parameters inducing double Dutch. In Van den Broecke, M., van Heuven, V., & Zonneveld, (Eds.), Sound structures, studies for Atonie Cohen. Dordrecht, Holland: Foris, 1983, 5369.Google Scholar
Elsendoorn, B. A. G. Production and perception of English vowel duration by Dutch speakers of Engslish. In Cohen, A. & Van den Broecke, M. (Eds.), Proceedings of the tenth international congress on the phonetic sciences. Dordrecht, Holland: Foris, 1984, 673–76.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. Fricatives in child language acquisition. In Heilmann, L. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 11th international congress of linguists. Bologna: Mulino, 1975, 647–64.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. Phonological processes. In Greenberg, J. (Ed.), Universals of human language. Vol. 2. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1978, 403–42.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E.Phonetic approximation in second language acquisition. Language Learning, 1980, 30, 117–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. English speakers learn to suppress stop devoicing. In Chametzky, R., Hirzel, R., & Tuite, K. (Eds.), Papers from the 18th regional meeting. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society, 1982, 111–22.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. The detection of foreign accent by American listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1984, in press. (a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. The production and perception of foreign language speech sounds. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Human communication and its disorders, Vol. 1. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex Publishing, 1985.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Hammond, R.Mimicry of non-distinctive phonetic differences between language varieties. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1982, 5, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Hillenbrand, J.Limits on pronunciation accuracy in foreign language speech production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1984, 76, 708–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Port, R. F.Cross-language phonetic interference: Arabic to English. Language and Speech, 1981, 24, 125–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S.Language transfer and universal grammatical relations. Language Learning, 1982, 29, 327–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gladstein, L. A., Membrino, M. E., & Flege, J. E.The effect of semantic context on Americans’ perception of pronunciation errors by Vietnamese speakers of English. Unpublished manuscript, 1982, Department of Linguistics, Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Harris, J.Spanish phonology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1969.Google Scholar
Hillenbrand, J., Ingrisano, D. R., Smith, B., & Flege, J. E.Perception of the voiced-voiceless contrast in syllable final stops. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1984, 76, 1826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodson, B., & Paden, E.Phonological processes which characterize unintelligible and intelligible speech in early childhood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1981, 46, 369–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D.Phonological disability in children. New York: Elsevier, 1976.Google Scholar
Ingram, D., Christensen, L., Veach, S., & Webster, B. The acquisition of word-initial fricatives and affricates in English by children between 2 and 6 years. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J., & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Child phonology, Vol. 1, Production. New York: Academic Press, 1980, 169–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, R.Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton, 1968.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D.Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: Evidence from acoustic studies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1976, 19, 421–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leopold, W.Speech development of a bilingual child, Vol. 2, Sound-learning in the first two years. New York: AMS Press, 1947/1970.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L.Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press, 1983.Google Scholar
Mack, M.Voicing dependent vowel duration in English and French: Monolingual and bilingual production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1982, 71, 173–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macken, M. A. Aspects of the acquisition of stop systems: A cross-linguistic perspective. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J., & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Child phonology, Volume 1, Production. New York: Academic Press, 1980, 143–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macken, M., & Barton, D.The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish: A phonetic and phonological study of word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 1980, 7, 433458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, M., & Ferguson, C. Phonological universals in language acquisition. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Native language and foreign language acquisition. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 1981, 110–30.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P., Hutchinson, J., & Lasater, S. The production of speech: Development and dissolution of motoric and pre-motoric processes. In Long, J. & Baddeley, A. (Eds.), Attention and performance, 9. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1981.Google Scholar
Major, R. C.The ontogeny of second language phonology. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Linguistics, Washington State University, Pullman, 1984.Google Scholar
Moskowitz, B. A.The acquisition of fricatives: A study in phonetics and phonology. Journal of Phonetics, 1975, 3, 141–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulford, R. C., & Hecht, B. F.Learning to speak without an accent: Acquisition of a second-language phonology. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 1980, 18, 1674.Google Scholar
Port, R., & Mitleb, F. Phonetic and phonological manifestations of the voicing contrast in Arabic-accented English. Research in Phonetics, 1980, 1, 137166. Bloomington: Department of Linguistics, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Raphael, L.Preceding vowel duration as a cue to the perception of the voicing characteristic of word-final consonants in American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1972, 51, 1,296–1,303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shockey, L.Phonetic and phonological properties of connected speech. Ohio State Working Paper in Linguistics, 1974, 17.Google Scholar
Smith, B. L.Effects of place of articulation and vowel environment on “voiced” stop consonant production. Glossa, 1978, 12, 163175.Google Scholar
Smith, B. L., & Stoel-Gammon, C.A longitudinal study of the development of stop consonant production in normal and Down’s Syndrome children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1983, 48, 114–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, N.The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1973.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. English speakers’ acquisition of Dutch syntax. In Winitz, H. (Ed.). Native language and foreign language acquisition. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 1981, 235–50.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., & Hoefnagel-Höhle, M.The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development, 1978, 49, 1, 114–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E.The acquisition of some Dutch morphological rules. Journal of Child Language, 1980, 7, 539–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, K.A detailed analysis of articulation responses of “normal” first grade children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1963, 6, 277–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stampe, D. A. Dissertation on natural phonology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1969/1979. (Available from the Indiana University Linguistics Club, Bloomington, IN.)Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. Design features of speech sound systems. In MacNeilage, P. F. (Ed.), The production of speech. New York: Springer, 1983, 247–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarone, E. E. The phonology of interlanguage. In Richards, J. (Ed.), Understanding second and foreign language learning. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House, 1978, 1533.Google Scholar
Tarone, E. E.Some influences on the syllable structure of interlanguage phonology. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 1980, 28, 139–52.Google Scholar
Tarone, E. E. The role of the syllable in interlanguage phonology. In Eliasson, S. (Ed.), Theoretical issues in contrastive phonology. Heidelberg: Julius Groos, 1981.Google Scholar
Walsh, H.On certain practical inadequacies of distinctive feature systems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1974, 59, 3243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weismer, G., Dinnsen, D., & Elbert, M.A study of the voicing distinction associated with omitted, word-final stops. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1981, 46, 320–27.Google Scholar
Williams, L. Phonetic variation as a function of second-language learning. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J., & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Child phonology, Volume 2, Perception. New York: Academic Press, 1980, 185216.Google Scholar
Wode, H.The L2 acquisition of /r/. Phonetica, 1977, 34, 200–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wode, H. Language-acquisitional universals: A unified view of language acquisition. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Native language and foreign language acquisition. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 1981, 218–35.Google Scholar