Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:28:59.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Syntactic comprehension in young poor readers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Suzanne T. Smith*
Affiliation:
Neurolinguistics Laboratory, Massachusetts General Hospital
Paul Macaruso
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories
Donald Shankweiler
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories
Stephen Crain
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories
*
Suzanne T. Smith, MGH Neurolinguistics Laboratory, 15 River Street, Boston, MA 02108

Abstract

Children with specific reading disability fail to understand some complex spoken sentences as well as good readers. This investigation sought to identify the source of poor readers' comprehension difficulties. Second-grade good and poor readers were tested on spoken sentences with restrictive relative clauses in two experiments designed to minimize demands on working memory. The methodological innovations resulted in a high level of performance by both reader groups, demonstrating knowledge of relative clause structure. The poor readers' performance closely paralleled that of the good readers both in pattern of errors and in awareness of the pragmatic aspects of relative clauses. The findings suggest that limitations in processing account for comprehension difficulties displayed by some poor readers in previous investigations.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. B. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and activation (Vol. 4). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Bohannon, J. N., Warren-Leubecker, A., & Hepler, N. (1984). Word order awareness and early reading. Child Development, 55, 15411548.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1986). Syntactic awareness in verbal performance from preschool to fifth grade. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 285308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. (1981). Deficient syntactic control in poor readers: Is a weak phonetic memory code responsible? Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 201212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chall, J. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Chomsky, C. (1969). The acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 3, 7584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, R. (1972). Speech and reading. In Kavanagh, J. F. & Mattingly, I. G. (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye: The relationships between speech and reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusions, and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 7584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crain, S., & Fodor, J. D. (forthcoming). Competence and performance in child language. In Dromi, E. (Ed.), Language and cognition: A developmental perspective. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Crain, S., & Shankweiler, D. (1988). Syntactic complexity and reading acquisition. In Davison, A. & Green, G. (Eds.), Linguistic complexity and text comprehension: Readability issues reconsidered. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Shankweiler, D., Macaruso, P., & Bar-Shalom, E. (in press). Working memory and sentence comprehension: Investigations of children with reading disorder. In Vallar, G. & Shallice, T. (Eds.), Impairments of short-term memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Crowder, R. G. (1978). Audition and speech coding in short-term memory. In Requin, J. (Ed.), Attention and performance, VII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Hirsch, K., Jansky, J., & Langford, W. (1966). Predicting reading failure. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. J., Satz, P., & Scholes, R. (1981). Developmental changes in the linguistic performance correlates of reading achievement. Brain and Language, 13, 7890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest-Pressley, D. L., & Waller, T. G. (1984). Cognition, metacognition and reading. New York: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In Cooper, W. E. & Walker, E. (Eds.). Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. (1988). Grammaticality judgments and reading skill in grade 2. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 7394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, M. A., Johnson, C. S., & Muehl, S. (1970). Oral language production in relation to reading achievement among select second graders. In Bakker, D. J. & Satz, P. (Eds.), Specific reading disabilities: Advances in theory and method. Rotterdam: Rotterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, S. (1980). Psycholinguistic bases of reading disability: A study in sentence comprehension. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The City University of New York.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H., & Tavakolian, S. (1982). Competence and processing in children's grammar of relative clauses. Cognition, 11, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamburger, H., & Crain, S. (1982). Relative acquisition. In Kuczaj, S. II (Ed.), Language development: Vol. 1. Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hamburger, H., & Crain, S. (1984). Acquisition of cognitive compiling. Cognition, 17, 85136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. (1983). Specific reading retardation and working memory: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 311342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M. (1989). Reading is hard just because listening is easy. In von Euler, C. (Ed.), Wenner-Gren International Symposium Series: Brain and reading. Hampshire, England: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, I. Y. (1983). A language-oriented view of reading and its disabilities. In Myklebust, H. J. (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (Vol. 5). New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Mann, V. A., Shankweiler, D., & Werfelman, M. (1982). Children's memory for recurring linguistic and nonlinguistic material in relation to reading ability. Cortex, 18, 367375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, V. A., Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1980). Children's memory for sentences and word strings in relation to reading ability. Memory and Cognition, 8, 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, V. A., Shankweiler, D., & Smith, S. T. (1984). The association between comprehension of spoken sentences and early reading ability: The role of phonetic representation. Journal of Child Language, 11, 627643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattis, S., French, J. H., & Rapin, I. (1975). Dyslexia in children and young adults: Three independent neuropsychological syndromes. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 17, 150163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P., & Flood, J. (1981). Language development, reading/writing problems and remediation. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 31, 1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morice, R., & Slaghuis, W. (1985). Language performance and reading ability at 8 years of age. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 141160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, H. E., & Warrington, E. K. (1980). An investigation of memory function in dyslexic children. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 487503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Richardson, E., & DiBenedetto, B. (1986). Decoding skills test. Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Roit, M. L. (1980). Syntactic development in normals and dyslexies: An empirical examination of two theoretical explanations of dyslexia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. S. (1967). The grammar of english predicate construction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). The structure and acquisition of reading II: The reading process and the acquisition of the alphabet principle. In Reber, A. S. & Scarborough, D. L. (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reading disorders: A modular approach. Cognition, 24, 139168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Brady, S., & Macaruso, P. (in press). Identifying the causes of reading disability. In Gough, P. B. (Ed.), Reading acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y. (1972). Misreading: A search for causes. In Kavanagh, J. F. & Mattingly, I. G. (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye: The relationships between speech and reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Smith, S. T., & Mann, V. A. (1984). Repetition and comprehension of spoken sentences by reading-disabled children. Brain and Language, 23, 241257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheldon, A. (1974). The role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clauses in English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 272281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. T., Mann, V. A., & Shankweiler, D. (1986). Spoken sentence comprehension by good and poor readers: A study with the Token Test. Cortex, 22, 627632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, C. L., Cairns, H. S., & Zurif, E. B. (1984). Sentence comprehension limitations related to syntactic deficits in reading-disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 305322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tavakolian, S. L. (1981). The conjoined-clause analysis of relative clauses. In Tavakolian, S. L. (Ed.), Language acquisition and linguistic theory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Pruzek, R. M., Steger, J. A., & Meshoulam, U. (1973). Immediate visual recall in poor and normal readers as a function of orthographic-linguistic familiarity. Cortex, 9, 370386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogel, S. A. (1975). Syntactic abilities in normal and dyslexic children. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, R., & Rabinovitch, M. S. (1971). Sentence structure and retention in good and poor readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 2530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiig, E. H., & Roach, M. A. (1975). Immediate recall of semantically varied sentences by learning disabled readers. Perceptual Motor Skills, 40, 119125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar