Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:04:00.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spontaneous language markers of Spanish language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2007

GABRIELA SIMON-CEREIJIDO
Affiliation:
San Diego State University and University of California, San Diego
VERA F. GUTIÉRREZ-CLELLEN
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego

Abstract

Spanish-speaking (SS) children with language impairment (LI) present with deficits in morphology and verb argument structure. These language areas may be useful for clinical identification of affected children. This study aimed to evaluate the discrimination accuracy of spontaneous language measures with SS preschoolers to tease out what combination of grammatical measure(s) were responsible for the LI deficits, and to determine the role of verb argument structure and syntactic complexity in identifying SS children with LI. Two sets of experiments were conducted on the spontaneous language samples of SS preschoolers with and without LI using discriminant function analyses. The study found that (a) mean length of utterance in words (MLUW) and ungrammaticality index in combination are fair to good discriminators of preschoolers with LI; (b) a morphology model combining correct use of articles, verbs, and clitics fairly discriminates LI children but may miss children whose language has limited syntactic complexity; and (c) semantic–syntactic complexity measures, such as MLUW, theme argument omissions, and ditransitive verb use, should be considered in the assessment of Spanish LI. The children who were bilingual and Spanish dominant in the study were classified as accurately as the Spanish-only children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson R. T. (1999). Loss of gender agreement in L1 attrition: Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal, 23, 389408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson R. T., & Souto S. M. (2005). The use of articles by monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 621647.Google Scholar
Badia I., & Serra M. (2005). Verb use patterns of children with specific language impairment: An experimental study of event representations. Paper presented at the X International Congress for the Study of Child Language.
Bedore L., & Leonard L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore L., & Leonard L. B. (2005). Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 195225.Google Scholar
Bedore L. M., & Leonard L. B. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 11851192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.
Bortolini U., Caselli M. C., Deevy P., & Leonard L. B. (2002). Specific language impairment in Italian: The first steps in the search for a clinical marker. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 7793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch L., & Serra M. (1997). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Child language disorders in a cross-linguistic perspective. Paper presented at the Fourth Symposium of the European Groups on Child Language Disorders.
Campos H. (1986). Indefinite object drop. Lingusitic Inquiry, 17, 354359.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden G., & Hesketh A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38, 251263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden G., & Jones M. (1997). Verb use in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 12981313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal D. (1987). Towards a “bucket” theory of language disability: Taking account of interaction between linguistic levels. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 1, 722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deevy P., & Leonard L. B. (2004). The comprehension of wh-questions in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 802815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan C. A., Campbell T. F., Paradise J. L., Feldman H. M., Janosky J. E., Pitcairn D. N., et al. (1999). Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 14321443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn M., Flax J., Sliwinski M., & Aram D. (1996). The use of spontaneous language measures as criteria for identifying children with specific language impairment: An attempt to reconcile clinical and research incongruence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 643654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gavin W. J., Klee T., & Membrino I. (1993). Differentiating specific language impairment from normal language development using grammatical analysis. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 7, 191206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein B. (2004). Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish-English speakers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Grela B. G., & Leonard L. B. (1997). The use of subject arguments by children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 11, 443453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grela B. G., & Leonard L. B. (2000). The influence of argument-structure complexity on the use of auxiliary verbs by children with SLI. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 43, 11151125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen V., Restrepo M. A., & Simon-Cereijido G. (2006). Evaluating the discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12091223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen V., & Simon-Cereijido G. (2006). Evaluation of the discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Latino English-speaking children. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Gutiérrez-Clellen V. F., & Kreiter J. (2003). Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 267288.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen V. F., Restrepo M. A., Bedore L., Peña E. S., & Anderson R. (2000). Language sample analysis in Spanish speaking-children: Methodological considerations. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 8898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen V. F., Restrepo M. A., Silva M. R., & Del Castillo T. S. (2000). Cross linguistic profiles of Latino children exposed to a second language. Paper presented at the 2000 Symposium of Research in Child Language Disorders.
Jacobson P. F. (2002). Morphology in incipient bilingual Spanish-speaking preschool children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 2341.Google Scholar
Jewkes A. M. (2005). The power of the family: A longitudinal investigation of how the home environment influences preschool language development. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.
Johnston J. R. (2001). An alternate MLU calculation: Magnitude and variability of effects. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 156164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King G., & Fletcher P. (1993). Grammatical problems in school-age children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 7, 339352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klee T., Stokes S. F., Wong A. M.-Y., Fletcher P., & Gavin W. J. (2004). Utterance length and lexical diversity in Cantonese-speaking children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 13961410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee L. (1976). Normal and atypical semantic development. Unpublished manuscript, Northwestern University.
Leonard L., Bedore L., & Grela B. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 741753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Leonard L. B., & Eyer J. A. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 741754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard L. B., & Finneran D. (2003). Grammatical morpheme effects on MLU: “The same can be less” revisited. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 878888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loebell H., & Bock K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 41, 791824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson J. J., & Kamhi A. G. (1991). The effects of sampling conditions on sentence production in normal, reading-disabled, and language-learning disabled children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 549558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson J. J., & Kamhi A. G. (1992). Linguistic trade-offs in school-age children with and without language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 10641075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer M. (1973). Frog on his own. New York: Author.
Mayer M. (1974). Frog goes to dinner. New York: Author.
Miller J. F., & Chapman R. (2000). Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Madison, WI: Language Analysis Laboratory.
Montrul S. A. (2004). The acquisition of Spanish: Morphosyntactic development in monolingual and bilingual L1 acquisition and adult L2 acquisition(Vol. 37). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Müller N., & Hulk A. (2001). “Comment expliquez-vous?” Null objects in adults and children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 4953.Google Scholar
Payne A. C., Whitehurst G. J., & Angell A. L. (1994). The role of home literacy environment in the development of language ability in preschool children from low-income families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 427440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña E. D., Gutiérrez-Clellen V. F., Iglesias A., Goldstein B., & Bedore L. M. (2006). Bilingual English Spanish assessment (BESA). Unpublished manuscript.
Plante E., & Vance R. (1994). Selection of preschool language tests: A data-base approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 25, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 13981411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo M. A., & Gutiérrez-Clellen V. F. (2001). Article use in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 28, 433452.Google Scholar
Restrepo M. A., & Gutiérrez-Clellen V. F. (2004). Grammatical impairments in Spanish–English bilingual children. In B. Goldstein (Ed.), Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish–English speakers (pp. 213234). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Rice M., Ruff-Noll K., & Grimm H. (1997). An extended optional infinitive stage in German-speaking children with specific language impairment. Language Acquisition, 6, 255295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice M., & Wexler K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 12391257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice M., Wexler K., & Cleave P. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice M. L. (2003). A unified model of specific and general language delay: Grammatical tense as a clinical marker of unexpected variation. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 6395). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rice M. L., & Bode J. V. (1993). GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. First Language, 13 (37, Pt. 1), 113131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice M. L., & Oetting J. B. (1993). Morphological deficits of children with SLI: Evaluation of number marking and agreement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 12491257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz Torrent M. (2002). Los verbos en niños con trastorno de lenguaje [Verb use in children with language impairment]. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, XXII, 100110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serra M., Serrat E., Solé R., Bel A., & Aparici M. (2000). La adquisición del lenguaje [Language acquisition]. Barcelona: Editorial Ariel S.A.
Snyder P., & Lawson S. (1993). Evaluating results using corrected and uncorrected effect size estimates. Journal of Experimental Education, 61, 334349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick B. G., & Fidell L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics. New York: HarperCollins.
Thordardottir E. T., & Weismer E. (2002). Verb argument structure weakness in specific language impairment in relation to age and utterance length. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16, 233250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
US Census Bureau. (2000). Differences in population by race and Hispanic or Latino origin, for the United States: 1990–2000. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.
van der Lely H. K. J. (1998). SLI in children: Movement, economy and deficits in the computational syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7, 211224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler K. (2003). Lennenberg's dream: Learning, normal language development, and specific language impairment. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wong A. M.-Y., Leonard L. B., Fletcher P., & Stokes S. F. (2004). Questions without movement: A study of Cantonese-speaking children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 14401453.Google Scholar
Zagona K. (2002). The syntax of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.