Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:55:54.637Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2009

ERIN K. ROBERTSON
Affiliation:
University of Québec at Montréal
MARC F. JOANISSE*
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Marc Joanisse, Department of Psychology, Social Sciences Centre, Room 7336, University of Western Ontario, London, ON N6A 5C2, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We examined spoken sentence comprehension in school-age children with developmental dyslexia or language impairment (LI), compared to age-matched and younger controls. Sentence–picture matching tasks were employed under three different working memory (WM) loads, two levels of syntactic difficulty, and two sentence lengths. Phonological short-term memory (STM) skills and their relation to sentence comprehension performance were also examined. When WM load was minimized, the LI group performed more poorly on the sentence comprehension task compared to the age-matched control group and the dyslexic group. Across groups, sentence comprehension performance generally decreased as the WM load increased, but this effect was somewhat more pronounced in the dyslexic group compared to the age-matched group. Moreover, both the LI and dyslexic groups showed poor phonological STM compared to the age-matched control group, and a significant correlation was observed between phonological STM and sentence comprehension performance under demanding WM loads. The results indicate subtle sentence processing difficulties in dyslexia that might be explained as resulting from these children's phonological STM limitations.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Archibald, L. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Visuospatial immediate memory in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 265277.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1981). The concept of working memory. A review of its current status and probable future development. Cognition, 10, 1723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. (1989). Test of Reception of Grammar. Manchester: University of Manchester, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2006). Distinct genetic influences on grammar and phonological short-term memory deficits: Evidence from 6-year old twins. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 5, 158169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Bishop, S. J., Bright, P., James, C., Delaney, T., & Tallal, P. (1999). Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 155168.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioral marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders, 36, 421432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Associated reading skills in children with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Reading and Writing, 19, 7798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read—A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Adolf, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 13781396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42, 741748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Bree, E., Rispens, J., & Gerrits, E. (2007). Non-word repetition in Dutch children with (a risk of) dyslexia and SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 935944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., Gernsbacher, M., Utman, J., & Dronkers, N. (2001). Language deficits, localization, and grammar: Evidence for a distributive model of language breakdown in aphasics and normals. Psychological Review, 108, 759788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 11361146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationship between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 563582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goulandris, N. K., Snowling, M. J., & Walker, I. (2000). Is dyslexia a form of specific language impairment? A comparison of dyslexia and language impaired children as adolescents. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 103120.Google Scholar
Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 3060.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R., & Kamhi, A. G. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of utterances of language impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 30, 6586.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122149.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A. G., & Catts, H. W. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 337347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leech, R., Aydelott, J., Symons, G., Carnevale, J., & Dick, F. (2007). The effect of semantic and attentional distractors on syntactic processing in typical development and adulthood. Developmental Science, 10, 794813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 3554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, V. A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 592599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, V. A., Shankweiler, D., & Smith, S. T. (1984). The association between comprehension of spoken sentences and early reading ability: The role of phonetic representation. Journal of Child Language, 11, 627643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, S. M., Heath, S. M., & Mengler, E. D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 869874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C. F., Bishop, D. V. M., & Briscoe, J. (2001). Production of English finite verb morphology: A comparison of SLI and mild to moderate hearing impairment, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 165178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rispens, J., & Been, P. H. (2007). Subject–verb agreement and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia and SLI. A closer look. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 293305.Google Scholar
Rispens, J. E., Roeleven, S., & Koster, C. (2004). Sensitivity to subject–verb agreement in spoken language in children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 333347.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 17281743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A. M., Liberman, A. M., Brady, S. A., et al. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6, 149155.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Smith, S. T., & Mann, V. A. (1984). Repetition and comprehension of spoken sentences by reading disabled children. Brain and Language, 23, 241257.Google Scholar
Smith, S. T., Macaruso, P., Crain, S., & Shankweiler, D. (1989). Syntactic comprehension in young poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 429454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2000). Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 587600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading-disabled children. A regression-based test of the phonological-core-variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 2453.Google Scholar
Tumner, W. E. (1989). The role of language-related factors in reading disability. In Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I. Y. (Eds.), Phonology and reading disability. Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 91131). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1994). Canonical linking rules: Forward versus reverse linking in normally developing and specifically language impaired children. Cognition, 51, 2972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs. adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98, 243272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Harris, M. (1990). Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 101117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Stollwerck, L. (1997). Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245290.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.Google Scholar
WagnerR., K. R., K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 7387.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. A. (1992). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. A. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1987). Speech perception in severely disabled and average reading children. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 4861.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. W. (1989). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar