Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T19:20:00.247Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from lowand middle-income families

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Rose I. Arriaga
Affiliation:
Harvard University
Larry Fenson*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
Terry Cronan
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
Stephen J. Pethick
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
*
Developmental Psychology Lab, MC-4910, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

This study compared the language skills in a group of very low-income toddlers with those of a middle-income sample matched on age and sex. The assessment instrument was the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) for toddlers, a parent report form. The scores for the low-income group were strikingly lower on the three key indices evaluated: size of expressive vocabulary, age of appearance of word combinations, and complexity of utterances. The entire lowincome distribution was shifted about 30% toward the lower end of the middle-income distribution for both productive vocabulary and grammatical development. The magnitude of these income/ social class effects was larger than reported in most prior reports for children in this age range. This finding underscores the cautionary note issued by the CDI developers, which states that the published CDI norms, based on a middle-class sample, may not be directly applicable to low-income samples.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, R., Wasserman, O., & Seidman, S. (1990). Children with congenital anomalies: The preschoolperiod. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 15, 327345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arriaga, R. I., Hicks, M. J., Cronan, T. A., & Fenson, L. (1992, March). Language comprehensionassessment: Mothers’ predictions and children's performance. Poster presented at the Stu-dent Research Conference,San Diego State University,San Diego.Google Scholar
Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G. (1983). Characteristics of adult speech whichpredict children's language development. Journal of Child Language, 10, 6584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertenthal, B. I. (1991). Special section: Canalization of behavioral development. DevelopmentalPsychology, 27, 339.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., & Lock, S. L. (1988). Home environment and school performance: A ten-year follow-up and examination of three models of environmental action. Child Devel-opment, 59, 852867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: Characteristicsand consequences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (6–7, Serial No. 153).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronan, T. A., Walen, H. R., & Cruz, S. G. (1994). The effects of community based literacy trainingon Head Start parents. Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 248258.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: The contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A., Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Diamond, L., & le-Furgy, W. G. (1988). Screening for developmental handicaps: Outcomes froman early childhood screening program. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 8, 4359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thai, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in earlycommunicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (5, Serial No. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L.Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thai, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide andtechnical manual. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Sweet, M., & Jimerson, T. (1996, April). Assessment of vocabulary comprehension:Parent judgment and child performance. Poster presented at the International Conference onInfant Studies,Providence,RI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia Coll, C. T. (1990). Developmental outcome of minority infants: A process-oriented look atour beginnings. Child Development, 61, 270289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottfried, A. W. (1984). Home environment and early cognitive development: Longitudinal re-search. Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., Hart, B., Kamps, D., Terry, B., Arreaga-Mayer, C., Atwater, J., Walker, D., Risley, T., & Delquadri, J. C. (1992). Out of the laboratory and into the community. American Psychologist, 47, 14641474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, C. R., Terry, B., Utley, C. A., & Montagna, D. (1993). Achievement, placement, andservices: Middle school benefits of classwide peer tutoring used at the elementary school. School Psychology Review, 22, 497516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullo, D. F. (1988). A comparative study of adolescent and older mothers’ knowledge of infantabilities. Child Study Journal, 18. 223231.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. DevelopmentalPsychology. 28, 10961105.Google Scholar
Hess, R. D., & Shipman, V. (1965). Early experience and the socialization of cognitive modes inchildren. Child Development. 36, 869886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingshead, A. (1965). Two-Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale UniversitySociology Department.Google Scholar
Klineberg, O. (1963). Negro-white differences in intelligence test performance: A new look at anold problem. American Psychologist. 18, 198203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Largo, R. H., Graf, S., Kubdu, S., Hunziker, U., & Molinari, L. (1990). Predicting developmentaloutcome at school age from infant tests of normal, at risk, and retarded infants. Develop-mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 3045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laosa, L. M. (1984). Ethnic, socioeconomic, and home language influences upon early performanceon measures of abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 11781198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, V. W., & Shipley, E. F. (1996). Parental speech to middle- and working-class childrenfrom two racial groups in three settings. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 233255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lesser, G., Fifer, G., & Clark, D. (1965). Mental abilities of children from different social classesand cultural groups. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 30 (4, Serial No. 102).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCall, R. B. (1981). Nature-nuture and the two realms of development: A proposed integrationwhich respect to mental development. Child Development, 52, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, D. A. (1978). Manual for the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Mishra, R. K., & Mishra, B. (1992). Acquisitional hierarchy of wh-words: Impact of family types. Psycho-Lingua, 22, 716.Google Scholar
Morisset, C. E., Barnard, K. E., Greenberg, M. T., Booth, C. L., & Spieker, S. J. (1990). Environ-mental influences on early language development: The context of social risk. Developmentand Psychopathotogy, 2, 127149.Google Scholar
Rescoria, L. (1989). The language development survey: A screening trial for delayed language intoddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorder, 54, 587599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznick, S. (1990). Visual preference as a test of infant word comprehension. Applied Psycholin-guistics, 11, 145166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1985). Family and school influences on cognitive development. Journal of Child Psy-chology and Psychiatry, 26, 683704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, L. S. (1982). Reproductive, perinatal, and environmental factors as predictors of the cognitive and language development of preterm and full-term infants. Child Development, 53, 969973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L., Ulvund, S. E., & Lindemann, R. (1994). Very low birth weight infants (<1501 g) atdouble risk. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 15, 713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J., & Vorster, J. (1976). Mothers'speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research, 5, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, J. H., Blondis, T., & Brady, L. (1988). Motor and sensory abilities with normal and academi-cally at-risk children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 227238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St. Pierre, R., & Swartz, J. P. (1995). The Even Start Family Literacy Program. In Smith, S. & Sigel, I. E. (Eds.), Advances in applied developmental psychology: Volume 9. Two generationprograms for families in poverty: A new intervention strategy (pp. 3766). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
St. Pierre, R., Swartz, J., Gamse, B., Murray, S., Deck, D., & Nickel, P. (1995). National evaluationof the Even Start Family Literacy Program (03 23, 1995). Washington, DC: U.S. De-partment of Education.Google Scholar
Vohr, B. R., GarciaColl, C., & Oh, W. (1989). Language and neurodevelopment outcome of low-birth weight infants at three years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 582590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, D., Greenwood, C, Hart, B., & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based onearly language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 65, 606621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A., & Carter, B. W. (1988). Reading and growth in metalinguistic awareness:Relations to socioeconomic status and reading readiness skills. Child Development, 59, 728742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. (1981). Language through interaction: The study of language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. (1985). Language development in the preschool years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar