Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T05:28:20.005Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reading and language processing. John M. Henderson, Murray Singer and Fernanda Ferreira (Eds.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 1995. PP. 351.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Csaba Pléh
Affiliation:
Eötvös University

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Carlson, G. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1988). Thematic roles and language comprehension. Syntax and Semantics. 21, 263288.Google Scholar
Carlson, G. N. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1989). Linguistic structure in language processing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A., & Bower, G. (Eds.). (1990). The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 25. Inferences and text comprehension. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1962). Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist, 17, 748762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. (1964). A preliminary functional model for language behavior. International Audiology, 3, 216225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Part II. Psychological Review, 89, 6094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed