Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:14:40.487Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pragmatic language disorder and perspective taking in autistic speakers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Joanne Volden*
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
R. F. Mulcahy
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
G. Holdgrafer
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
*
J. Volden, Audiology and Speech Sciences, University of British Columbia, 5804 Fairview Ave., Vancouver, British Columbia V6J 4X9, Canada

Abstract

The relationship between pragmatic referential communication skill and the cognitive ability to assess and assume another person's conceptual viewpoint was investigated in the autistic population. Ten high functioning autistic adolescents and young adults were matched for age and sex to normally developing controls and given referential communication and perspectivetaking tasks that had been previously demonstrated to be of comparable complexity. The groups were selected to be similar in terms of language skill.But despite their intact, elementary perspective-taking skills, the autistic subjects displayed significant communicative dysfunction. This suggests that factors other than a deficiency in the development of a “theory of mind” are significant contributors to the social communicative disorder associated with autism.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.) [DSM-111R]. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 379402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285297.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J. (1989). The theory of mind hypothesis of autism: Explanation, evidence and assessment. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 181198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happe, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story character's thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holdgrafer, C., & Campbell, T. (1986). Children's comprehension of intonation as a marker for discourse topic collaboration. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 373384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Smith, P., Boucher, J., Harris, P. (1994). Comprehension of pretense in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 433456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242246.Google Scholar
LeCouteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrager, M., & McLennan, J. (1989). Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 325339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K., Landry, S., Hughes, S., Hall, S., & McEvoy, S. (1988). Speech acts and the pragmatic deficits of autism. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 593604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loveland, K., & Tunali, B. (1991). Social scripts for conversational interactions in autism and Down's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 177186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K., Tunali, B., McEvoy, R., Kelley, M. (1989). Referential communication and response adequacy in autism and Down's syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 301313.Google Scholar
Reed, T. (1994). Performance of autistic and control subjects on three cognitive perspective-taking tasks. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 24, 5366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, R., & Patterson, G. (1983). Perspective taking and referential communication: The question of correspondence reconsidered. Child Development, 54, 10051014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schopler, E., Reichler, R., Renner, B. (1986). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) for diagnostic screening and classification of autism. New York: Irving ton.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). Psycholinguistic approaches to language and communication in autism. In Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. (Eds.), Communication problems in autism (pp. 6987). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1987). On the nature of a language acquisition disorder: The example of autism. In Kessel, F. (Ed.), The development of language and language researchers (Essays in honor of Roger Brown) (pp. 249267). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). A psycholinguistic perspective on language development in the autistic child. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism: New directions in diagnosis, nature and treatment (pp. 92115).New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, R. (1994). A second look at second order belief attribution in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 577586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerer, J. (1989). The early development of autistic children: Implications for defining primary deficits. In Dawson, G. (Ed.) Autism: Nature, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 7591). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Volden, J., Holdgrafer, G., Mulcahy, R. (1993). Pragmatic language dysfunction in autism: “Competence or performance”? Journal of Clinical Speech Language Studies, 13,3653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volden, J., & Lord, C. (1991). Neologisms and idiosyncratic language in autistic speakers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 109130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A., & Prutting, C. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364377.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (1987). lCD-10 1986 draft of chapter 5 categories F00-F99. Mental, behavioural and developmental disorders. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar