Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:25:32.296Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On Rhyme, language, and children's reading

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Judith A. Bowey*
Affiliation:
The University of Queensland
*
Judith A. Bowey, Department of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia 4067, Queensland, Australia

Abstract

This article provides a commentary on Bryant MacLean, and Bradley (1990). It is argued that their results do not differ greatly from those reported by Bowey and Patel (1988), with the discrepancies in the outcomes of multiple regression analyses reflecting the relative size of simple correlations, which are probably attributable to differences in the design of the two studies. These differences can only be settled by future replication work. Methodological suggestions are made for maximizing the generalizability of such work.

Type
Dialogue
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bentler, P. M. (1989). EQS Structural Equations Program Manual. Los Angeles, CA: BMDP Statistical Software.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1988). Metalinguistic functioning in children. Geelong, Australia: Deakin University Press.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & deBhal, M. C. (1989). Selectively prompted speech repairs in stages I to V. Unpublished manuscript, University of Queensland.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Francis, J. (in press). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Patel, R. K. (1988). Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 367383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Differences in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746747.Google Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., MacLean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407428.Google Scholar
Bryant, P., & Goswami, U. (1987). Phonological awareness and learning to read. In Beech, J. R. & Colley, A. M. (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to reading (pp. 213243). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L. L. (1990). Rhyme, language, and children's reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 237252.Google Scholar
Content, A., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., & Bertelson, P. (1986). Phonetic segmentation in prereaders: Effect of corrective information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 4972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Share, D. L., MacLean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Phonological confusability in short term memory for sentences as a predictor of reading ability. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 393400.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A. G., & Catts, H. W. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 337347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamhi, A. G., Lee, R. F., & Nelson, L. K. (1985). Word, syllable, and sound awareness in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 207212.Google Scholar
MacLean, M., Bryant, P., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255281.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24, 4564.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a series of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323331.Google Scholar
Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (1982). Tests of Language Development – Primary. Austin, TX: Services for Professional Educators.Google Scholar
Pratt, C., Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Children's capacity to correct grammatical violations in sentences. Journal of Child Language, 11, 129141.Google Scholar
Read, C., Zhang, Y.-N., Nie, H.-Y., & Ding, B.-Q. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 3144.Google Scholar
Reynell, J. K., & Huntley, M. (1987). Developmental language scales (Rev. ed.). East Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (in press). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reading disorder: A modular approach. Cognition, 24, 139168.Google Scholar
Smith, C. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1982). Metalinguistic awareness and language development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 449468.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics (2nd ed.). New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R., & Wright, A. D. (1987). Syntactic awareness and reading acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 2534.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321363.Google Scholar
Weaver, P. A. (1979). Improving reading comprehension: Effects of sentence organization instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 15, 130146.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1967). Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. Cleveland, OH: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar