Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T03:45:47.118Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2012

KELLY BRIDGES*
Affiliation:
New York University and Nathan Kline Institute
ERIKA HOFF
Affiliation:
Florida Atlantic University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Kelly Bridges, Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, New York University, 665 Broadway, 9th Floor, New York, NY 10012. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Two separate studies examined older siblings’ influence on the language exposure and language development of US-born toddlers who were being raised in bilingual homes. The participants in Study 1 were 60 children between 16 and 30 months who had heard English and another language at home from birth; 26 had older siblings, and 34 did not. The participants in Study 2 were 27 children, assessed at 22 and 30 months, who had heard English and Spanish from birth; 14 had school-aged older siblings, and 13 did not. Both studies found that older siblings used English more in talking to the toddlers than did other household members and that toddlers with older siblings were more advanced in English language development. Study 2 also found that the presence of a school-aged older sibling increased mothers’ use of English with their toddlers and that toddlers without a school-aged older sibling were more advanced in Spanish than the toddlers with a school-aged older sibling. These findings contribute to a picture of the complex processes that shape language use in bilingual homes and cause variability in young children's bilingual development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E. (1975). Peer relations and the acquisition of language. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (Eds.), Friendship and peer relations (pp. 259292). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Lerner, R. M. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 9931028). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Center for Early Care and Education Research. (2010). Dual language learners. Retrieved from http://www.cecerdll.fpg.unc.edu Google Scholar
Caldas, S. J. (2006). Raising bilingual-biliterate children in monolingual cultures. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, A., & Wei, L. (2008). Individual differences in the lexical development of French–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11, 598618.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2009). Bilingual first language acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Bornstein, M. (2003). Balancing on the tightrope: Language use patterns in bilingual families with young children. Paper presented at the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism, Tempe, AZ, April 30–May 1.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 95116.Google Scholar
Garcia, E., & Jensen, B. (2009). Early educational opportunities for children of Hispanic origins (Social Policy Report 23, No. 2). Ann Arbor, MI: Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (Ed.). (2007). Language transmission in bilingual families in Wales. Cardiff: Welsh Language Board.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 213237.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Hoff, E. (2007). Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 107127). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K., & D'Andrea, D. (1992). Some properties of bilingual maintenance and loss in Mexican background high-school students. Applied Linguistics, 13, 7299.Google Scholar
Hammer, C. S., Miccio, A. W., & Rodriguez, B. L. (2004). Bilingual language acquisition and the child socialization process. In Goldstein, B. A. (Ed.), Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish–English speakers (pp. 2152). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2011). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22, 127.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 603629.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E., & Krueger, W. M. (1991). Older siblings as conversational partners. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 37, 465482.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ishizawa, H. (2004). Minority language use among grandchildren in multigenerational households. Sociological Perspectives, 47, 465483.Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Newton, T., Fenson, L., & Conboy, B. (2003). MacArthur Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas. User's Guide and Technical Manual. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Jia, G., & Aaronson, D. (2003). A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 131161.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K. (2002). Picture naming in early sequential bilinguals: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 45, 759771.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., & Martínez-Sussmann, C. (2002). Concurrent validity of caregiver/parent report measures of language for children who are learning both English and Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 993997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., Martínez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212224.Google Scholar
McCardle, P., & Hoff, E. (2006). An agenda for research on childhood bilingualism. In McCardle, P. & Hoff, E. (Eds.), Childhood bilingualism: Research on infancy through school age (pp. 157165). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 3043.Google Scholar
Morrison, F. J., Connor, C. M., & Bachman, H. J. (2006). The transition to school. In Dickinson, D. K. & Neuman, S. B. (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (2nd ed., pp. 375394). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. (Eds.). (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2011). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish–English bilingually developing 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 113125.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 4158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 18341849.Google Scholar
Scheele, A. F., Leseman, P. P. M., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 117140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squires, J., Potter, L., & Bricker, D. D. (1999). Ages and Stages Questionnaire: A parent completed, child monitoring system (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Vagh, S. B., Pan, B. A., & Mancilla-Martinez, J. (2009). Measuring growth in bilingual and monolingual children's English productive vocabulary development: The utility of combining parent and teacher report. Child Development, 80, 15451563.Google Scholar
Wang, Z.-L. (2008). Growing up with three languages: Birth to eleven. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007). The bilingual child: Early development and language contact. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Jin, X., Shen, X., Zhang, J., & Hoff, E. (2008). Correlates of early language development in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 145151.Google Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P. (2002). Sibling caregiving. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed., pp. 253286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar