Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T02:36:30.820Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Jan Edwards*
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
Margaret Lahey
Affiliation:
Emerson College
*
Department of Speech & Hearing Science, The Ohio State University, 110 Pressey Hall, 1070 Carmack Road, Columbus, OH 43210. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

To examine possible explanations of the reported inaccuracies of children with specific language impairment (SLI) on nonword repetition, we compared the repetitions of 54 children with SLI and their peers in terms of number and type of error as well as latency and duration of response. We found no evidence of differences between the groups in auditory discrimination or response processes, but we did find some evidence suggesting differences in either the formation or storage of phonological representations in working memory. Because repetition accuracy was significantly correlated with expressive, but not receptive, measures of language, we hypothesized that the problem lay with the nature of phonological representations in working memory and not with the ability to hold phonological information in working memory.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American National Standards Institute (1989). Specifications for audiometers (ANSI S3.6-1989). New York: ANSI.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Beckman, M. E., de Jong, K., Jun, S., & Lee, S. (1992). The interaction of coarticulation andprosody in sound change. Language and Speech, 35, 4558.Google Scholar
Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (1996). Lexical frequency effects on young children's imitative productions. Paper presented at the Fifth Conference on Laboratory Phonology,Evanston, IL.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 33, 164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioral marker forinherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 37, 391403.Google Scholar
Brady, S., Poggie, E., & Rapala, M. M. (1989). Speech repetition abilities in children who differ inreading skill. Language and Speech, 32, 109122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L., Sherbenor, R., & Johnson, S. (1990). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Catts, H. (1989). Speech production deficits in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech andHearing Disorders, 54, 422428.Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In Siegler, R. (Ed.), Chil-dren's thinking: What develops? (pp. 7396). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, lettersand numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C. (1987). Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speechand Hearing Research, 52, 218222.Google ScholarPubMed
Dollaghan, C., Biber, E., Campbell, T. F. (1993). Constituent syllable effects in a nonsense-wordrepetition task. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 10511054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C., Biber, E., Campbell, T. F. (1995). Lexical influences on nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 211222.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peahody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor, Ontario: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1993). Auditory lexical decisions in children and adults: An examinationof response factors. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 36, 9961003.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1996). Auditory lexical decisions of children with specific language impairment. Journal ofSpeech and Hearing Research, 39, 12631273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eguchi, S., & Hirsh, I. (1969). Development of speech sounds in children. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 257 (Supplement).Google ScholarPubMed
Elliott, L., & Hammer, M. (1988). Longitudinal changes in auditory discrimination in normal chil-dren and children with language learning problems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-ders, 53, 467474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, L. L., Hammer, M. A., & Evan, K. E. (1987). Perception of gated, highly familiar words bychildren, teenagers, and older adults. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 150157.Google Scholar
Elliott, L., Hammer, M. A., & Scholl, M. (1989). Fine grained auditory discrimination in normalchildren and children with language-learning problems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re-search, 32, 112119.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., & Hesketh, L. J. (1996). Lexical learning by children with specific languageimpairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speechand Hearing Research, 39, 177190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowl-edge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23, 8394.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology. 29, 770778.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. (1994). Children's phonological working memory: Contributions of long-term knowledge andrehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 672688.Google Scholar
Gathereole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in thedevelopment of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Lan-guage, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathereole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990a). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is diere a causal connec-tion? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathereole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990b). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young childrenlearning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathereole, S. E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Nonword repetition, phonological memory, andvocabulary: A reply to Snowling, Chiat, and Hulme. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 375379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathereole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. (1991). The influences of number ofsyllables and wordlikeness on children's repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathereole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.Google Scholar
German, D. (1986). Test of Word Finding. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, G, & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development ofshort-term span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241253.Google Scholar
James, D., Van Steenbrugge, W., & Chiveralls, K. (1994). Underlying deficits in language-disorderedchildren with central auditory processing difficulties. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 311328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1991). Processing time declines exponentially during childhood and adolescence. Develop-mental Psychology, 27, 259266.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and readingdisorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 337347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamhi, A., Catts, H., Mauer, D., Apel, K., & Gentry, B. (1988). Phonological and spatial processingabilities in language- and reading-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-ders, 53, 316327.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A., & Koenig, L. (1985). Metalinguistic awareness in normal and language-disordered children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 16, 199210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. M., & Kaufman, N. L. (1990). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1992). The biology of phonological development. In Ferguson, C. A.Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 6590). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D., & Fomer, L. L. (1980). Speech segment durations in sentence recitations by childrenand adults. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 157168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchner, D., & Klatsky, R. (1985). Verbal rehearsal and memory in language disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 556564.Google Scholar
Kirk, S., McCarthy, J., & Kirk, W. (1968). The Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Urban: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Lahey, M. (1988). Language disorders and language development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Lahey, M., & Bloom, L. (1994). Variability and language learning disorders. In Wallach, G. P.Butler, K. G. (Eds.), Language learning disabilities in school-age children and adolescents(pp. 354372). New York: Macrnillan.Google Scholar
Lahey, M., & Edwards, J. (1996). Why are children with specific language impairment slower thantheir peers in naming pictures? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 10811098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, L. (1971). Northwestern Syntax Screening Test. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., McGregor, K. K., & Allen, G. D. (1992). Grammatical morphology and speech percep-tion in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 10761085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, P. A., Feustel, T. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (1983). Capacity demands in short-term memory for synthetic and natural speech. Human Factors, 25, 1732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. (1984). Function and process in spoken word recognition. In Bouma, H. & Bouhis, D. (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 125150). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Merriman, W. E., & Schuster, J. M. (1991). Young children's disambiguation of object name refer-ence. Child Development, 62, 12881301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michas, I. C, & Henry, L. A. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabularyacquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 147163.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995a). Examination of phonological working memory in specifically lan-guage-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 355378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995b). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role ofphonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, W. D., Shea, S. L., & Aslin, R. N. (1989). Identification of vowels in “vowelless” syllablesby three-year-olds. Perception and Psychophysics, 46, 375383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neils, J., & Aram, D. (1986). Family history of children with developmental language disorders. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 655658.Google Scholar
Newcomer, P., & Hammill, D.. (1988). The Test of Language Development - Primary. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plante, E., Swisher, L., Kiernan, B., & Restrepo, M. A. (1993). Language matches: Illuminating orconfounding? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 772776.Google Scholar
Powell, R. P., & Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). Clumsiness and perceptual problems in children withspecific language impairment. Development Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 755765.Google Scholar
Raine, A., Hulme, C., Chadderton, H., & Bailey, P. (1991). Verbal short term memory span inspeech disordered children: Implications for articulatory coding in short-term memory. ChildDevelopment, 30, 415423.Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. G. (1995). Effect of familiarity on word duration in children's speech: A preliminaryinvestigation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 7684.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Sininger, Y. S., Klatzky, R. L., & Kirchner, D. M. (1989). Memory-scanning speed in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 289297.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43. 219234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Com-ments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, J., Poppen, R., & May, M. (1987). Effects of alterations of prosodic features on the sequencing performance of aphasic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 10, 849855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, D. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapidmovement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In Stelmach, G. E. (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 118150). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Sussman, J. E. (1993). Perception of formant transition cues to place of articulation in children withlanguage impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36. 12861299.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: Rate of auditory processing and selectiveimpairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 8393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Stark, R., & Mellits, D. (1985). Identification of language-impaired children on the basisof rapid perception and production skills. Brain and Language, 25, 314322.Google Scholar
Taylor, H. G., Lean, D., & Schwartz, S. (1989). Pseudoword repetition ability in learning-disabledchildren. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 203219.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. J., & Howard, D. (1993). Children with specific language impairment: Linguisticimpairment or short-term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36,11931207.Google Scholar
van Kleeck, A. (1994). Metalinguistic development. In Wallach, G. & Butler, K. (eds.), Language learn-ing disabilities in school-age children and adolescents (pp. 5398). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Watkins, R., Kelly, D. J., Harbers, H. M., & Hollis, W. (1995). Measuring children's lexical diver-sity: Differentiating typical and impaired language-learners. Journal of Speech and HearingResearch, 38. 13491355.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Stager, C. L. (1996). Developmental changes in infant speech perception andearly word learning: Is there a link? Paper presented at the Fifth Conference on LaboratoryPhonology,Evanston, IL.Google Scholar
Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F., & Ovrut, M. (1990). The timing of syllable repetitions in developmentaldyslexia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 281289.Google Scholar