Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:25:52.249Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Measures of phonological short-term memory and their relationship to vocabulary development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

S. E. Avons*
Affiliation:
University of Wollongong
Christopher A. Wragg
Affiliation:
University of Wollongong
Wragg L. Cupples
Affiliation:
University of Wollongong
William J. Lovegrove*
Affiliation:
University of Wollongong
*
Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, England
Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, England

Abstract

Vocabulary scores, word span, nonword repetition, rhyme detection, and articulation rate measures were obtained for a group of 30 preschool children with a mean age of 4;11. After allowing for age and intelligence, it was found that the best predictors of vocabulary were memory span, rhyme detection, and nonword repetition, accounting for 19%. 15%, and 13% of the variance, respectively. Of these children, 28 were tested again on the same measures 13 months later. At the later stage, memory span and rhyme scores again predicted vocabulary to a significant extent, but nonword repetition score did not. Of the measures taken initially, memory span and rhyme detection significantly predicted later vocabulary scores, whereas nonword repetition just failed to reach significance. Cross-lagged correlations showed (hat memory span and rhyme detection on the first test predicted later vocabulary after partialing out initial vocabulary scores. However, initial vocabulary scores did not predict later memory span (or rhyme detection) after partialing out the initial span (or rhyming) scores. Articulation rate was not significantly related to vocabulary at either age. The results are interpreted as indicating that the phonological store of working memory (required by word span, rhyme detection, and nonword repetition, but independent of articulation rates) contributes to vocabulary development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Avons, S. E., Wright, K. L., & Pammer, K. (1994). The word-length effect in probed and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 207231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (vol. 8, pp. 4790). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term shortage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of shortterm memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 14, 575589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L. (1980). Assessing reading difficulties: A diagnostic and remedial approach. London: Macmillan Education.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P. E., Maclean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N., Day, L., Saults, J. S., Keller, T. A., Johnson, T., & Floras, L. (1992). The role of verbal output time in the effects of word length on immediate memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C. A., Biber, M. E., & Campbell, T. F. (1995). Lexical influences on nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 211222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23, 8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Do young children rehearse? An individual differences analysis. Memory & Cognition, 22, 201207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991a). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in Children. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 387406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991b). Nonword repetition, phonological memory and vocabulary: A reply to Snowling, Chiat, and Hulme. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 375379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children's repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, L. A. (1991). The effects of word length and phonemic similarity in young children's shortterm memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A, 3552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hitch, G. J., Halliday, M. S., & Littler, J. E. (1989). Item identification time and rehearsal rate as predictors of memory span in children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 321337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., & Tordorff, V. (1989). Working memory development: The effects of speech rate, word length and acoustic similarity on serial recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 7287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michas, I. C., & Henry, L. A. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 147163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolson, R. I. (1981). The relationship between span and processing speed. In Friedman, M., Das, J. P., & O'Connor, N. (Eds.), Intelligence and learning (pp. 179184) New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1992). Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effect of phonological similarity and item length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 4767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1956). Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Alban, J., Ellis, A. W., & Brown, G. D. A. (1994). Effects of word frequency and age of acquisition on short-term memory span. Memory & Cognition, 22, 695701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathereole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., Smith, A., & Thomas, J. (1994). The effects of phonological similarity and list length on children's categorization performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 160180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, J. T. L., Scott, J. H., & Power, K. G. (1987). Developmental differences in the span of visual memory for pattern. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 249255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W. (1984). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery. Allen, TX: DIM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar