Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:57:26.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Letters, sounds, and symbols: Changes in children's understanding of written language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Ellen Bialystok*
Affiliation:
York University
*
Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele St., North York, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada

Abstract

Children between 3–5 years who knew the alphabet but could not read were given three tasks. In the first, they decided which of two words was longer when the word pairs were presented orally, in writing, or accompanying pictures. In the second, they “read” a word when it accompanied a picture of the named object and then again when it was placed with a picture of a different object. Finally, they were given a set of plastic letters with which they could create their own words. Although all the children had explicit knowledge of letters and sounds, they lacked symbolic knowledge of how letters represent sounds. This symbolic knowledge, it is claimed, is a precondition to learning to read.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bowey, J. A. (1986). Syntactic awareness in relation to reading skill and ongoing reading comprehension monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 282299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read – a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. R. (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. E., & Goswami, U. (1987). Phonological awareness and learning to read. In Beech, J. R. & Colley, A. M. (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to reading. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clay, M. M. (1985). The early detection of reading difficulties: A diagnostic survey (3rd ed.). Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children's minds. Glasgow: Fontana.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. (1983). The development of literacy: A complex psychological problem. In Coulmas, F. & Ehlich, K. (Eds.), Writing in focus. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. (1984). The underlying logic of literacy development. In Goelman, H., Oberg, A., & Smith, F. (Eds.), Awakening to literacy. Exeter, NH: Heineman.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. London: Heinemann Educational.Google Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1975). Analysing spoken language into words, syllables, and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 331342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garton, A., & Pratt, C. (1989). Learning to be literate: The development of spoken and written language. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Holden, M. H., & MacGinitie, W. H. (1972). Children's conceptions of word boundaries in speech and print. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 551557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landsmann, L. T., & Levin, I. (1987). Writing in four- to six-year olds. Representation of semantic and phonetic similarities and differences. Journal of Child Language, 14, 127144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundberg, I. (1978). Aspects of linguistic awareness related to reading. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R. J., & Levelt, W. J. M. (Eds.), The child's conception of language. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Papandropoulou, I., & Sinclair, H. (1974). What is a word? Experimental studies of children's ideas on grammar. Human Development, 17, 241258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., Cancino, H., De Temple, J., & Schley, S. (in press). Giving formal definitions: A linguistic or metalinguistic skill? In Bialystok, E. (Ed.), Language processing by bilingual children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition. In Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valtin, R. (1984). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children learning to read and write. In Downing, J. & Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Watson, A. J. (1984). Cognitive development and units of print in early reading. In Downing, J. & Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar