Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:34:07.507Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The functions of imitation in child language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Zita Réger*
Affiliation:
Mta Nyelvtudományi Intézete
*
Zita Réger, MTA Nyelvtudományi Intézete, Szentháromság utca 2., H-1250 Budapest, Hungary

Abstract

Earlier studies demonstrating the discourse function of imitation have been inconclusive for methodological reasons. In the present study three discourse-related formal aspects of model-imitation pairs are analysed longitudinally in successive samples from two Hungarian children. Examination of the types of modification introduced by the children revealed an unbroken developmental trend leading to lexically coherent conversational replies. As development progressed, questions were imitated significantly more often than nonquestions. The children's selection from multiword utterances showed convergence on a language specific discourse rule. Imitation served (i)lexical learning, (ii) the phonological approximation of adult word forms, and (iii)grammatical learning, in this temporal order. The high rate of grammatically and lexically progressive items in the earliest sample was attributed to nonselective echolalia, supposedly a genetically earlier form of imitation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Blasdell, R., & Jensen, P. (1970). Stress and word position as determinants of imitation in first-language learners. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 13, 193202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., Hood, L., & Lightbown, P. (1974). Imitation in language development: If, when, and why. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 380420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L., & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 521552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In Lenneberg, E. H. (Ed.), New directions in the study of language (pp. 131161). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1974). Performing without competence. Journal of Child Language, 1, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. (1977). What's the use of imitation? Journal of Child Language, 4, 341358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, W. R., & Brown, M. B. (1979). The BMDP-P series. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. (1972). Einführung in die Textlinguistik. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Du, Preez P. (1974). Units of information in the acquisition of language. Language and Speech, 17, 369376.Google Scholar
Ékiss, K. (1977). Topic and focus in Hungarian syntax. Montreal Working Papers in Linguistics, 8, 142.Google Scholar
Ékiss, K. (1978). A magyar mondatok egy szintaktikai modellje [A syntactic model of Hungarian sentences]. Nyelvtudományi Közlemények, 80, 261286.Google Scholar
Ékiss, K. (1981). Topic and focus: The operators of the Hungarian sentence. Folia Linguistica, 15, 305330.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. (1964). Imitation and structural change in children's language. In Lenneberg, E. H. (Ed.), New directions in the study of language (pp. 163189). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Kainz, F. (1943). Psychologie der Sprache: II. Stuttgart: Ferdinand Enke.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E. (1969). The role of intonation in the acquisition of language. Unpublished dissertation. Cornell University. Cited in P. Menyuk (1971). The acquisition and development of language. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Kassai, I. (1979). Gyermeknyelvi dallamminták [Melodic patterns in child language]. Magyar Fonetikai Füzetek, 4, 147171.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1974). Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language, 1, 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1977). Making it last: Repetition in children's discourse. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.), Child discourse (pp. 125138). New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J. (1968). Introduction to theoretical linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McTear, M. F. (1978). Repetition in child language: Imitation or creation. In Campbell, R., & Smith, P. (Eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (pp. 293311). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. L. (1977). Processes and products of imitation: Additional evidence that imitation is progressive. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 6, 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moerk, E. L., & Moerk, C. (1979). Quotations, imitations and generalizations. Factual and methodological analyses. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2, 4372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Natrella, M. G. (1963). Experimental statistics. National Bureau of Standards Handbook 91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preyer, W. (1905). Die Seele des Kindes, 6. Leipzig: Th. Grieben's.Google Scholar
Ramer, A. L. H. (1976). The function of imitation in child language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 700717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Risley, T. R., & Reynolds, N. J. (1970). Emphasis as a prompt for verbal imitation. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis. 3, 185190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodd, L. J., & Braine, M. D. S. (1970). Children's imitations of syntactic constructions as a measure of linguistic competence. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 430443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, J. (1973). Interpretation and imitation in early language development. In Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.), Constraints on learning (pp. 427443). London: Academic.Google Scholar
Shapiro, T., Roberts, A., & Fish, B. (1970). Imitation and echoing in young schizophrenic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 9, 548567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1981). The uses of imitation. Journal of Child Language, 8, 205212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1983). Saying it again: The role of expanded and deferred imitations in language acquisition. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Children's language: Vol. 5. New York: Gardner.Google Scholar
Stine, E. L., & Bohannon, J. N. (1983). Imitations, interactions, and language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10, 589603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varga, L. (1985). Intonation in the Hungarian sentence. In Kenesei, I. (Ed.), Approaches to Hungarian: Vol. one. Data and descriptions (pp. 205224). Szeged: JATE.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1900). Völkerpsychologie: Band, I. Die Sprache. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar