Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:44:10.207Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Expressions of past time by normal and language-impaired children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Mary Evelyn Moore*
Affiliation:
Illinois State University
Judith R. Johnston
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
*
Mary Evelyn Moore, Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology 4720, Illinois State University, Normal, IL 61761-6901

Abstract

This study investigated possible developmental asynchrony in children with specific language impairment (SL1). This was accomplished by comparing the development of temporal expressions of past reference in two linguistic domains. Verb tense and temporal adverbs served as representatives of the morphological and lexical domains. A cross-sectional design was used with four groups of subjects (N = 48) from 3 to 5 years old. Three groups had normal language and one group had impaired language. Results indicated that the performances of SLI children were similar to those of their 3-year-old language peers on the verb tasks and similar to the 4-year-olds on the adverb tasks. Such differential delays provide direct experimental evidence for asynchronous language development in SLI children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ames, L. (1946). The development of the sense of time in the young child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 68, 97125.Google ScholarPubMed
Aram, D. & Nation, J. (1975). Patterns of language behavior in children with developmental language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 18, 229241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, A. & Chamberlain, I. (1904). Studies of the child. Pedogogical Seminary, 11, 264291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corson, D. (1989). Adolescent lexical difference in Australia and England by social group. Journal of Educational Research, 82(3), 146157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromer, R. (1968). The development of temporal reference during the acquisition of language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
De Villiers, J. & de Villiers, P. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 7484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B., Johnston, J. & L., Brandl (1993). Relation between mental age and vocabulary development among children with mild mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 97, 541546.Google ScholarPubMed
Hiskey, M. (1966). Hiskey-Nebraska Test of Learning Aptitude. Lincoln, NE: College View Printers.Google Scholar
Illinois State Board of Education. (1990). Speech and Language Technical Assistance Manual (Appendix D). Springfield: State of Illinois.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1982). The language disordered child. In Lass, N., McReynolds, L., Northern, J. & Yoder, D. (Eds.), Speech, language, and hearing (Vol. 2, pp. 780801). Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1985). Cognitive prerequisites: The evidence from children learning English. In Dan, Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 2, pp. 9611004). Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1986). Semantic analyses of language sample data [workshop]. Presented at the convention of the American Speech and Hearing Association, Detroit.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. & Kamhi, A. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 6585.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. & Schery, T. (1976). The use of grammatical morphemes by children with communication disorders. In Morehead, D. & Morehead, A. (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 239258). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Kemp, J. (1983). The timing of language intervention for the pediatric population. In Miller, J., Yoder, D. & Schiefelbusch, R. (Eds.) Contemporary issues in language intervention (pp. 183195). ASHA Reports 12. Rockville, MD: American Speech Language Hearing Association.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. & Boston, R. (1982). The nature and development of personal temporal-reference systems. In Kuczaj, S. (Ed.), Language development (Vol. 2, pp. 365395). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. (1991). Specific language impairment as a clinical category. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 22, 6668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Schwartz, R., Chapman, K., Rowan, L., Prelock, P., Terrell, B., Weiss, A. & Messick, C. (1982). Early lexical acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 554563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratsos, M. (1974). Preschool children's use of definite and indefinite articles. Child Development, 45, 446455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morehead, D. & Ingram, D. (1973). The development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 16, 330345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsen, D. (1972). English adverbials. Paris: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prutting, C., Gallagher, T. & Mulac, A. (1975). The expressive portion of the NSST compared to a spontaneous language sample. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 40, 4048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. & Svartvik, J. (1972). A grammar of contemporary English. New York: Seminar Press.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 4, pp. 128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schery, T. (1980). Correlates of language development in language disordered children: An archival study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Claremont McKenna College Graduate School, Claremont, CA.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. & Welsh, C. (1973). Elicited imitation as a research tool in developmental psycholinguistics. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 485496). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smith, C. (1978). The syntax and interpretation of temporal expressions in English. Linguistics and Philosophy, 2, 4399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. (1980). The acquisition of time talk: Relation between child and adult grammars. Journal of Child Language, 7,263278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snedecor, G. & Cochran, W. (1980). Statistical methods. Ames: Iowa State University Press.Google Scholar
SPSS-X, Inc. (1988). User's guide (3rd ed.). Chicago, 111: SPSS-X, Inc.Google Scholar
Steckol, K. & Leonard, L. (1979). The use of grammatical morphemes by normal and language impaired children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 12, 291301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, T. & Owen, D. (1986). The influence of paternal social class on intelligence and educational level in male adoptees and non-adoptees. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Hardy, J. & Hein, H. (1991). Predicting poor-communication status in preschool children using risk factors present at birth. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 10961105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weist, R. (1986). Tense and aspect. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (2nd ed., pp. 356374). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. (1985). Language development in preschool years. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar